Jump to content

Digit ratio

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This is an old revision of this page, as edited by 35.10.159.70 (talk) at 17:35, 17 November 2009 (Evidence of androgen effect on digit ratio). The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.

The digit ratio is the ratio of the lengths of different digits or fingers typically measured from the bottom crease where the finger joins the hand to the tip of the finger. It has been suggested by some scientists that the ratio of two digits in particular, the 2nd (index finger) and 4th (ring finger), is affected by exposure to androgens e.g. testosterone while in the uterus and that this 2D:4D ratio can be considered a crude measure for prenatal androgen exposure, with lower 2D:4D ratios pointing to higher androgen exposure.

2D:4D is sexually dimorphic: in males, the second digit tends to be shorter than the fourth, and in females the second tends to be the same size or slightly longer than the fourth. This trait may be better considered 'sexually differentiated' rather than 'sexually dimorphic' in recognition of the fact that the effect size is fairly small (2D:4D distributions of the two sexes overlap to a great degree), especially compared to other sexually dimorphic traits e.g. height.

History of digit ratio research

That a greater proportion of men have shorter index fingers than ring fingers than do women was noted in the scientific literature several times through the late 1800s [1], with the statistically significant sex difference established by 1930 [2], after which time the sex difference appears to have been largely forgotten or ignored. In 1983 Dr Glenn Wilson of King's College, London published a study examining the correlation between assertiveness in women and their digit ratio.[3] This was the first study to examine the correlation between digit ratio and a psychological trait within members of the same sex.[citation needed] Wilson proposed that skeletal structure and personality were simultaneously affected by sex hormone levels in utero.[3] In 1998, John T. Manning and colleagues reported the sex difference in digit ratios was present in two-year-old children [4]. Digit ratio research has since exploded, as perusal of citations at the end of this article will attest. In 2002 Manning of the University of Liverpool published a book summarizing all such research on the topic to that point[5], asserting that prenatal testosterone affects digit ratios and their psychological correlates.

A 2009 study in Biology Letters argues: "Sexual differences in 2D : 4D are mainly caused by the shift along the common allometric line with non-zero intercept, which means 2D : 4D necessarily decreases with increasing finger length, and the fact that men have longer fingers than women,"[6] which may be the basis for the sex difference in digit ratios and/or any putative hormonal influence on the ratios.

Evidence of androgen effect on digit ratio

  • Women with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH), which results in elevated androgen levels before birth, have lower, more masculinized 2D:4D on average[7][8].
  • Males with CAH have more masculine (smaller) digit ratios than control males [7][8], which also suggests that prenatal androgens affect digit ratios, since amniocentesis samples show that prenatal levels of testosterone are in the high normal range in males with CAH, while levels of the weaker androgen androstenedione are several fold higher than in control males [9][10][11]. These measures indicate that males with CAH are exposed to greater prenatal concentrations of total androgens than are control males.
  • Digit ratio in men correlates with genetic variation in the androgen receptor gene.[12] Men with genes that produce androgen receptors that are more sensitive to testosterone have lower, more masculine, digit ratios.
  • XY individuals with androgen insensitivity syndrome due to a dysfunctional gene for the androgen receptor present as women and have feminine digit ratios on average, as would be predicted if androgenic hormones affect digit ratios. This finding also demonstrates that the sex difference is unrelated to the Y chromosome per se.[13]
  • The sex difference in 2D:4D is present before birth in humans[14][15], which rules out any social influences that might affect digit growth differentially in the two sexes. Because all somatic sex differences in mammals to date have been found to be due to either androgenic masculinization or effects of the sex chromosomes, and as the AIS finding rules out a role for sex chromosomes in the sex difference in digit ratios,the prenatal sexual dimorphism also indicates that androgen acts before birth to affect digit ratios.
  • The ratio of testosterone to estradiol measured in 33 amniocentesis samples correlates with the child's subsequent 2D:4D ratio.[16] The effect of a child's sex is confusingly controlled for in this study.[original research?]
  • In pheasants, the ratio of the 2nd to 4th digit of the foot has been shown to be influenced by manipulations of testosterone in the egg.[17]

There is evidence that this reflects fetal exposure to the hormones testosterone[18] and estrogen.[citation needed]

Several studies present evidence that digit ratios are heritable[19][20].

Explanation of the digit ratio effect

It is not clear why digit ratio ought to be influenced by prenatal hormones. There is evidence of other similar traits, e.g. otoacoustic emissions and arm-to-trunk length ratio, which show similar effects. Hox genes responsible for both digit and penis development [21] have been implicated in this pleiotropy. Direct effects of sex hormones on bone growth might be responsible, either by regulation of Hox genes in digit development or independently of such genes.

Geographic/Ethnic variation in 2D:4D

Manning and colleagues have shown that 2D:4D ratios vary greatly between different ethnic groups.[22][23] This variation is far larger than the differences between sexes, in Manning's words "There’s more difference between a Pole and a Finn than a man and a woman."[This quote needs a citation] The variation appears to be correlated with latitude, such that more northerly populations have higher digit ratios.

Correlation between digit ratio and traits

Some authors suggest that digit ratio correlates with health, behavior, and even sexuality in later life. Below is a non-exhaustive list of some traits that have been either demonstrated or suggested to correlate with digit ratio.

Physiology and disease

  • Sperm counts[24]
  • Heart disease[25]
  • Obesity & Metabolic syndrome[26]

Psychological disorders

Physical and competitive ability

  • Skiing[33]
  • Soccer ability[34]
  • Sporting ability in females[19]
  • Financial trading[35]

Cognition and personality

  • Assertiveness in women[3]
  • Spatial ability[36]
  • Aggression[28][37]
  • Masculinity of Handwriting[38]
  • Perceived 'dominance' and masculinity of man's face[39][40]
  • Personality[41][42][43]
  • Exam scores: a higher ratio is correlated with higher exam scores among male students[17][44]
  • Musical ability[45]

Sexual orientation

Transsexualism

  • A recent study in Germany has found a correlation between digit ratio and male to female transsexualism. Male to female transsexuals (Transwomen) were found to have a higher digit ratio than control males, but one that was comparable to control females.[58]

Digit ratio and handedness, autism, other immune diseases

There is some evidence that testosterone facilitates the differentiation of the brain both prenatally and postnatally. There have been many extensions of this, such as the Geschwind–Galaburda hypothesis, that immune diseases[59] and autism[60] are related to prenatal testosterone, this also explaining why more men are left-handed, autistic, etc. than women. Prenatal exposure to testosterone is thought to promote the development of the right-hemisphere and increase the incidence of sinistrality. As such low 2D:4D was found to be associated with improved left-hand performance.[22][61]

Digit ratio and development

There is some evidence that 2D:4D ratio may also be indicative for human development and growth. Ronalds et al. (2002) showed that men who had an above average placental weight and a shorter neonatal crown-heel length had higher 2D:4D ratios in adult life.[citation needed] Moreover, studies about 2D:4D correlations with face shape suggest that testosterone exposure early in life may set some constraints for subsequent development. Prenatal sex steroid ratios (in terms of 2D:4D) and actual chromosomal sex dimorphism were found to operate differently on human faces, but affect male and female face shape by similar patterns.[62] However, exposure to very high levels of testosterone and/or estrogen in the womb may have also negative effects. Fink et al. (2004) found that men with low (indicating high testosterone) and women with high (indicating high estrogen) 2D:4D ratios express lower levels of facial symmetry.[63]

Digit ratio and palaeolithic hand stencils

It is generally assumed that creating cave art was a male behavior even though there is little evidence to substantiate this.[citation needed] 2D:4D is being used alongside other methods to help sex Palaeolithic hand stencils found in European and Indonesian caves.[64][65][66]

Digit ratio research in animals

  • Dennis McFadden and collaborators have demonstrated sexual dimorphism in hind limb digit ratio in a number of great apes, including gorillas and chimpanzees.[50]
  • Emma Nelson and Susanne Shultz are currently investigating how 2D:4D relates to primate mating strategies and the evolution of human sociality.[67]
  • Sexual dimorphism in hind limb 2D:4D has been demonstrated in mice by two studies by both John Manning and Marc Breedlove's research groups. There is some evidence to suggest that this effect is not seen in all mouse strains.[citation needed]
  • Nancy Burley's research group has demonstrated sexual dimorphism in zebra finches, and found a correlation between digit ratio in females and the strength of their preference for sexually selected traits in males.[citation needed]
  • Front limb D2:D3 has shown to be influenced by prenatal alcohol exposure in female rats[citation needed]
  • Alžbeta Talarovičová and collaborators found in rats that elevated testosterone during the prenatal period can influence 4D length, the 2D:4D ratio, and open field motor activity.[68]
  • Peter L. Hurd, Theodore Garland, Jr., and their students have examined hindlimb 2D:4D in lines of mice selectively bred for high voluntary wheel-running behavior (see experimental evolution). These high-runner mice exhibit increased 2D:4D. This apparent "feminization" is opposite to the relation seen between 2D:4D and physical fitness in human beings, and is difficult to reconcile with the idea that 2D:4D is a clear proxy for prenatal androgen exposure in mice. The authors suggest that 2D:4D may more accurately reflect effect of glucocorticoids or other factors that regulate any of various genes.[69]

See also

Notes

  1. ^ Ecker A (1875). "Einige Bemerkungen über einen Schwankenden Charakter in den Hand des Menschen[Some remarks about a varying character in the hand of humans]". Archiv fur Anthropologie. 8: 68–74.
  2. ^ George R (1930). "Human finger types". Anatomical Record. 46: 199–204.
  3. ^ a b c Wilson, Glenn D. (1983). "Finger-length as an index of assertiveness in women". Personality and Individual Differences. 4 (1): 111–2. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(83)90061-2.
  4. ^ Manning JT, Scutt D, Wilson J, Lewis-Jones DI (1998). "The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: a predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone and oestrogen". Hum Reprod. 13: 3000–3004. PMID 9853845.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  5. ^ Manning, John T. (2002). Digit ratio: a pointer to fertility, behavior, and health. New Brunswick, N.J: Rutgers University Press. ISBN 978-0-8135-3030-7.
  6. ^ Kratochvíl L, Flegr J (2009). "Differences in the 2nd to 4th digit length ratio in humans reflect shifts along the common allometric line". Biology Letters. 5 (5): 643–6. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2009.0346. PMID 19553247. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  7. ^ a b Brown WM, Hines M, Fane BA, Breedlove SM (2002). "Masculinized finger length patterns in human males and females with congenital adrenal hyperplasia" (PDF). Hormones and Behavior. 42 (4): 380–6. doi:10.1006/hbeh.2002.1830. PMID 12488105. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  8. ^ a b Okten A, Kalyoncu M, Yariş N (2002). "The ratio of second- and fourth-digit lengths and congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency". Early Human Development. 70 (1–2): 47–54. doi:10.1016/S0378-3782(02)00073-7. PMID 12441204. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  9. ^ Pang S, Levine LS, Cederqvist LL, Fuentes M, Riccardi VM,Holcombe JH, Nitowsky HM, Sachs G, Anderson CE, Duchon MA,Owens R, Merkatz I, New MI (1980). "Amniotic fluid concentrations of delta5 and delta4 steroids in fetuses with congenital adrenal hyperplasia". J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 51: 223–229. PMID 6447160.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  10. ^ Dorr, H. G., and Sippell, W. G. (1993). "Prenatal dexamethasone treatment in pregnancies at risk for congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency: Effect on midgestational amniotic fluid steroid levels". J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 76: 117–120. PMID 8421074.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  11. ^ LWudy, S. A., Dorr, H. G., Solleder, C., Djalali, M., and Homoki, J. (1999). "Profiling steroid hormones in amniotic fluid of midpregnancy by routine stable isotope dilution/gas chromatography­ mass spectrometry: Reference values and concentrations in fetuses at risk for 21-hydroxylase deficiency". J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 84: 2724–2728. PMID 10443667. {{cite journal}}: soft hyphen character in |title= at position 115 (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  12. ^ Manning, John T.; Bundred, Peter E.; Newton, Darren J.; Flanagan, Brian F. (2003). "The second to fourth digit ratio and variation in the androgen receptor gene". Evolution and Human Behavior. 24: 399–405. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(03)00052-7.
  13. ^ Berenbaum SA, Bryk KK, Nowak N, Quigley CA, Moffat S (2009). "Fingers as a marker of prenatal androgen exposure". Endocrinology. 150 (11): 5119–24. doi:10.1210/en.2009-0774. PMID 19819951. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  14. ^ Malas MA, Dogan S, Evcil EH, Desdicioglu K. (2006). "Fetal development of the hand, digits and digit ratio (2D:4D)". Early Hum Dev. 82: 469–475. PMID 16473482.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  15. ^ Galis F, Ten Broek CM, Van Dongen S, Wijnaendts LC (2009). "Sexual Dimorphism in the Prenatal Digit Ratio (2D:4D)". Arch Sex Behav. 38: Mar 20. [Epub ahead of print]. PMID 19301112.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  16. ^ Lutchmaya S, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Knickmeyer R, Manning JT (2004). "2nd to 4th digit ratios, fetal testosterone and estradiol". Early Human Development. 77 (1–2): 23–8. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2003.12.002. PMID 15113628. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  17. ^ a b Romano M, Leoni B, Saino N (2006). "Examination marks of male university students positively correlate with finger length ratios (2D:4D)". Biological Psychology. 71 (2): 175–82. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.03.006. PMID 15978716. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  18. ^ McIntyre MH (2006). "The use of digit ratios as markers for perinatal androgen action". Reproductive Biology and Endocrinology. 4: 10. doi:10.1186/1477-7827-4-10. PMC 1409789. PMID 16504142.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  19. ^ a b Paul SN, Kato BS, Hunkin JL, Vivekanandan S, Spector TD (2006). "The big finger: the second to fourth digit ratio is a predictor of sporting ability in women". British Journal of Sports Medicine. 40 (12): 981–3. doi:10.1136/bjsm.2006.027193. PMID 17008344. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  20. ^ Gobrogge, K.L., S.M.Breedlove & K.L.Klump (2008). "Genetic and environmental influences on 2d:4d finger length ratios: a study of monozygotic and dizygotic male and female twins". Archives Sexual Behavior. 37: 112–118. PMID 18074216.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  21. ^ Dickman S. (1997). "HOX gene links limb, genital defects". Science. 275 (5306): 1568–9. PMID 9072822. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  22. ^ a b Manning JT, Barley L, Walton J; et al. (2000). "The 2nd:4th digit ratio, sexual dimorphism, population differences, and reproductive success. evidence for sexually antagonistic genes?". Evolution and Human Behavior. 21 (3): 163–183. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00029-5. PMID 10828555. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  23. ^ Manning JT, Stewart A, Bundred PE, Trivers RL (2004). "Sex and ethnic differences in 2nd to 4th digit ratio of children". Early Human Development. 80 (2): 161–8. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2004.06.004. PMID 15500996. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  24. ^ Manning JT, Scutt D, Wilson J, Lewis-Jones DI (1998). "The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: a predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone and oestrogen". Human Reproduction. 13 (11): 3000–4. doi:10.1093/humrep/13.11.3000. PMID 9853845. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  25. ^ Manning JT, Bundred PE (2001). "The ratio of second to fourth digit length and age at first myocardial infarction in men: a link with testosterone?". British Journal of Cardiology. 8 (12): 720–3. ISSN 0969-6113.
  26. ^ Fink B, Manning JT, Neave N (2006). "The 2nd-4th digit ratio (2D:4D) and neck circumference: implications for risk factors in coronary heart disease". International Journal of Obesity. 30 (4): 711–4. doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.0803154. PMID 16261185. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  27. ^ Manning JT, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Sanders G (2001). "The 2nd to 4th digit ratio and autism". Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 43 (3): 160–4. doi:10.1017/S0012162201000317. PMID 11263685. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  28. ^ a b Bailey AA, Hurd PL (2005). "Finger length ratio (2D:4D) correlates with physical aggression in men but not in women". Biological Psychology. 68 (3): 215–22. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.05.001. PMID 15620791. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |laydate= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |laysource= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |laysummary= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  29. ^ Arató M, Frecska E, Beck C, An M, Kiss H (2004). "Digit length pattern in schizophrenia suggests disturbed prenatal hemispheric lateralization". Progress in Neuro-psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 28 (1): 191–4. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2003.09.020. PMID 14687873. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  30. ^ McFadden, D., Westhafer, J.G., Pasanen, E.G., Carlson, C.L., and Tucker, D.M. (2005). "Physiological evidence of hypermasculinization in boys with the inattentive subtype of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)". Clinical Neuroscience Research. 5: 233–245.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  31. ^ Stevenson JC, Everson PM, Williams DC, Hipskind G, Grimes M, Mahoney ER. (2007). "Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms and digit ratios in a college sample". Am J Hum Biol. 19: 41–50. PMID 17160985.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  32. ^ Martel, M.M, K.L.Gobrogge, S.M.Breedlove & J.T.Nigg (2008). "Masculinized finger length ratios of boys, but not girls, are associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder". Behavioral Neuroscience. 122: 273–281. PMID 18410167.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  33. ^ Manning, John (2002). Digit ratio: a pointer to fertility, behavior, and health. New Brunswick, N.J: Rutgers University Press. ISBN 978-0-8135-3030-7.[page needed]
  34. ^ Manning JT, Taylor RP (2001). "Second to fourth digit ratio and male ability in sport: implications for sexual selection in humans". Evolution and Human Behavior. 22 (1): 61–69. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00063-5. PMID 11182575. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  35. ^ Coates JM, Gurnell M, Rustichini A (2009). "Second-to-fourth digit ratio predicts success among high-frequency financial traders". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 106 (2): 623–8. doi:10.1073/pnas.0810907106. PMC 2626753. PMID 19139402. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |laydate= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |laysource= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |laysummary= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  36. ^ van Anders SM, Hampson E (2005). "Testing the prenatal androgen hypothesis: measuring digit ratios, sexual orientation, and spatial abilities in adults". Hormones and Behavior. 47 (1): 92–8. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.09.003. PMID 15579270. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  37. ^ Benderlioglu Z, Nelson RJ (2004). "Digit length ratios predict reactive aggression in women, but not in men". Hormones and Behavior. 46 (5): 558–64. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.06.004. PMID 15555497. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  38. ^ Beech, John R.; Mackintosh, Isla C. (2005). "Do differences in sex hormones affect handwriting style? Evidence from digit ratio and sex role identity as determinants of the sex of handwriting". Personality and Individual Differences. 39 (2): 459–68. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.01.024. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  39. ^ Neave N, Laing S, Fink B, Manning JT (2003). "Second to fourth digit ratio, testosterone and perceived male dominance". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 270 (1529): 2167–72. doi:10.1098/rspb.2003.2502. PMC 1691489. PMID 14561281. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  40. ^ Burriss RP, Little AC, Nelson EC (2007). "2D:4D and sexually dimorphic facial characteristics". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 36 (3): 377–84. doi:10.1007/s10508-006-9136-1. PMID 17203400. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  41. ^ Austin, Elizabeth J.; Manning, John T.; McInroy, Katherine; Mathews, Elizabeth (2002). "A preliminary investigation of the associations between personality, cognitive ability and digit ratio". Personality and Individual Differences. 33 (7): 1115–24. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00002-8. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  42. ^ Fink et al. 2004[verification needed]
  43. ^ Luxen, Marc F.; Buunk, Bram P. (2005). "Second-to-fourth digit ratio related to Verbal and Numerical Intelligence and the Big Five". Personality and Individual Differences. 39 (5): 959–66. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.03.016. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  44. ^ Brosnan MJ (2008). "Digit ratio as an indicator of numeracy relative to literacy in 7-year-old British schoolchildren". British Journal of Psychology. 99 (Pt 1): 75–85. doi:10.1348/000712607X197406. PMID 17535470. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |laydate= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |laysource= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |laysummary= ignored (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  45. ^ Sluming, Vanessa A.; Manning, John T. (2000). "Second to fourth digit ratio in elite musicians Evidence for musical ability as an honest signal of male fitness". Evolution and Human Behavior. 21 (1): 1–9. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00026-4. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  46. ^ a b Csathó A, Osváth A, Bicsák E, Karádi K, Manning J, Kállai J (2003). "Sex role identity related to the ratio of second to fourth digit length in women". Biological Psychology. 62 (2): 147–56. doi:10.1016/S0301-0511(02)00127-8. PMID 12581689. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  47. ^ McIntyre MH (2003). "Digit ratios, childhood gender role behavior, and erotic role preferences of gay men". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 32 (6): 495–6. doi:10.1023/A:1026054625638. PMID 14627046. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  48. ^ a b Williams TJ, Pepitone ME, Christensen SE; et al. (2000). "Finger-length ratios and sexual orientation" (PDF). Nature. 404 (6777): 455–6. doi:10.1038/35006555. PMID 10761903. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  49. ^ Tortorice JL (2002). "Written on the body: butch/femme lesbian gender identity and biological correlates". Rutgers University. OCLC 80234273. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  50. ^ a b c McFadden D, Shubel E (2002). "Relative lengths of fingers and toes in human males and females". Hormones and Behavior. 42 (4): 492–500. doi:10.1006/hbeh.2002.1833. PMID 12488115. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  51. ^ a b Hall LS, Love CT (2003). "Finger-length ratios in female monozygotic twins discordant for sexual orientation". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 32 (1): 23–8. doi:10.1023/A:1021837211630. PMID 12597269. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  52. ^ Rahman Q, Wilson GD (2003). "Sexual orientation and the 2nd to 4th finger length ratio: evidence for organising effects of sex hormones or developmental instability?". Psychoneuroendocrinology. 28 (3): 288–303. doi:10.1016/S0306-4530(02)00022-7. PMID 12573297. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  53. ^ Putz, David A.; Gaulin, Steven J. C.; Sporter, Robert J.; McBurney, Donald H. (2004). "Sex hormones and finger length: What does 2D:4D indicate?" (PDF). Evolution and Human Behavior. 25 (3): 182–99. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.03.005. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  54. ^ Rahman Q (2005). "Fluctuating asymmetry, second to fourth finger length ratios and human sexual orientation". Psychoneuroendocrinology. 30 (4): 382–91. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.10.006. PMID 15694118. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  55. ^ Kraemer B, Noll T, Delsignore A, Milos G, Schnyder U, Hepp U (2006). "Finger length ratio (2D:4D) and dimensions of sexual orientation". Neuropsychobiology. 53 (4): 210–4. doi:10.1159/000094730. PMID 16874008.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  56. ^ Wallien MS, Zucker KJ, Steensma TD, Cohen-Kettenis PT (2008). "2D:4D finger-length ratios in children and adults with gender identity disorder". Hormones and Behavior. 54 (3): 450–4. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.05.002. PMID 18585715. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  57. ^ Brown WM, Finn CJ, Cooke BM, Breedlove SM (2002). "Differences in finger length ratios between self-identified 'butch' and 'femme' lesbians" (PDF). Archives of Sexual Behavior. 31 (1): 123–7. doi:10.1023/A:1014091420590. PMID 11910785. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  58. ^ Schneider HJ, Pickel J, Stalla GK (2006). "Typical female 2nd-4th finger length (2D:4D) ratios in male-to-female transsexuals-possible implications for prenatal androgen exposure". Psychoneuroendocrinology. 31 (2): 265–9. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.07.005. PMID 16140461. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  59. ^ Geschwind and Galaburda, 1985[verification needed]
  60. ^ Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S (2004). "The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 34 (2): 163–75. doi:10.1023/B:JADD.0000022607.19833.00. PMID 15162935. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  61. ^ Fink B, Manning JT, Neave N, Tan U (2004). "Second to fourth digit ratio and hand skill in Austrian children". Biological Psychology. 67 (3): 375–84. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.03.012. PMID 15294393. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  62. ^ Fink B, Grammer K, Mitteroecker P; et al. (2005). "Second to fourth digit ratio and face shape". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 272 (1576): 1995–2001. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3179. PMC 1559906. PMID 16191608. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  63. ^ Fink, Bernhard; Manning, John T.; Neave, Nick; Grammer, Karl (2004). "Second to fourth digit ratio and facial asymmetry". Evolution and Human Behavior. 25 (2): 125–32. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(03)00084-9. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  64. ^ Snow, Dean R. (2006). "Sexual dimorphism in Upper Palaeolithic hand stencils". Antiquity. 80 (308): 390–404.
  65. ^ Chazine, Jean-Michel; Noury, Arnaud (2006). "Sexual Determination of Hand Stencils at the Masri II Cave". Inora Newsletter. 44: 21–6.
  66. ^ Nelson, Emma C.; Manning, John T.; Sinclair, Anthony G. M. (2006). "Using the length of the 2nd to 4th digit ratio (2D:4D) to sex cave art hand stencils: factors to consider" (PDF). Before Farming. 1 (6): 1–7.
  67. ^ Nelson, Emma. "Investigating relationships between the second-to-fourth digit ratio (2D:4D), social and bonding behaviours in non-human anthropoids". Retrieved October 29, 2009.[self-published source?]
  68. ^ Talarovičová A, Kršková L, Blažeková J (2009). "Testosterone enhancement during pregnancy influences the 2D:4D ratio and open field motor activity of rat siblings in adulthood". Hormones and Behavior. 55 (1): 235–9. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.10.010. PMID 19022257. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  69. ^ Yan RH, Malisch JL, Hannon RM, Hurd PL, Garland T (2008). "Selective breeding for a behavioral trait changes digit ratio". PLoS ONE. 3 (9): e3216. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0003216. PMC 2528935. PMID 18797502.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)