Jump to content

User:Jamesvoltage: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
No edit summary
No edit summary
Line 33: Line 33:
A similar output could be achieved by examining various threshold levels, so in a sense the addition of noise creates a new effective threshold for the measurement device.
A similar output could be achieved by examining various threshold levels, so in a sense the addition of noise creates a new effective threshold for the measurement device.


==History of Stochastic Resonance==

Stochastic resonance was first discovered in a study of the periodic recurrence of earth’s ice ages <ref name="Benzi1"> [http://www.iop.org/EJ/abstract/0305-4470/14/11/006] Benzi, R., Sutera, A., Vulpiani, A. “The mechanism of statistic resonance”, Journal of Physics A, Vol. 14. (1981). </ref> <ref name="Benzi2"> [http://www2.warwick.ac.uk/fac/sci/physics/research/cfsa/people/hnat/finalyearproj0910/benzi_on_climate_stochres.pdf] Benzi, R., Parisi, G., Sutera, A., Vulpiani, A. “A theory of stochastic resonance in climatic change”, SIAM Journal on Applied Mathematics, 1983. </ref> The theory developed out of an effort to understand how the earth's climate oscillates periodically between two relatively stable global temperature states. The conventional explanation was that variations in the eccentricity of earth's orbital path occurred with a period of 100,000 years and caused the average temperature to shift dramatically. The measured variation in the eccentricity had a relatively small amplitude compared to the dramatic temperature change, however, and stochastic resonance was developed to show that the weak eccentricity variation with added [[stochastic]] variation could cause the temperature to move in a nonlinear fashion between two stable dynamic states.


===Theoretical Grounding in Model Neurons===
===Theoretical Grounding in Model Neurons===


Not long after stochastic resonance was first discovered in a study of the periodic recurrence of earth’s ice ages <ref name="Benzi1"> [http://www.iop.org/EJ/abstract/0305-4470/14/11/006] Benzi, R., Sutera, A., Vulpiani, A. “The mechanism of statistic resonance”, Journal of Physics A, Vol. 14. (1981). </ref> <ref name="Benzi2"> [http://www2.warwick.ac.uk/fac/sci/physics/research/cfsa/people/hnat/finalyearproj0910/benzi_on_climate_stochres.pdf] Benzi, R., Parisi, G., Sutera, A., Vulpiani, A. “A theory of stochastic resonance in climatic change”, SIAM Journal on Applied Mathematics, 1983. </ref>, the phenomenon was demonstrated in a mathematical model of a single neuron using a dynamical systems approach <ref name="Bulsara"> [http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1758197] Bulsara, A., Jacobs, E.W., Zhou, T., Moss, F., Kiss, L. “Stochastic resonance in a single neuron model: theory and analog simulation”, Journal of Theoretical Biology (1991).</ref>. This analysis tested a pure tonal input with broadband noise and calculated the SNR from the power spectrum of the model neuron’s spike output. The characteristic peak for a particular input noise was apparent.
Not long after, the phenomenon was demonstrated in a mathematical model of a single neuron using a dynamical systems approach <ref name="Bulsara"> [http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1758197] Bulsara, A., Jacobs, E.W., Zhou, T., Moss, F., Kiss, L. “Stochastic resonance in a single neuron model: theory and analog simulation”, Journal of Theoretical Biology (1991).</ref>. This analysis tested a pure tonal input with broadband noise and calculated the SNR from the power spectrum of the model neuron’s spike output. The characteristic peak for a particular input noise was apparent.


==Animal Physiology Experiments==
==Animal Physiology Experiments==

Revision as of 22:30, 6 December 2009

Personal Information

Hello, my name is James, I'm a graduate student in psychology at Cornell University. I'm a member of Prof. Carl Hopkins's Neuroethology course this semester, and I will be working on a draft of my page "Neural Mechanisms of Stochastic Resonance" in this space.

What is stochastic resonance?

Stochastic resonance is a phenomenon that occurs when the effective detection threshold of a threshold measurement system is optimized for a non-zero value of input noise [1].

The three criteria that must be met for stochastic resonance to occur are:

  1. Nonlinear measurement device - output is on or off set by threshold
  2. Signal of interest - must be weak, periodic and at some point below threshold of measurement device
  3. Input noise - random variation added to signal of interest

Stochastic resonance occurs when these conditions combine in such a way that a certain average noise intensity results in maximized SNR. A time-averaged (or, equivalently, low-pass filtered) output due to signal of interest plus noise will yield an even better measurement of the signal in terms of SNR.

The threshold measurement creates harmonic distortion, in which unwanted frequency components of the output signal occur at integer multiples of the fundamental frequency where the amplitude of the harmonic decreases with its distance from the fundamental. Adding noise before measuring with the threshold causes this harmonic distortion to be whitened, or spread across the frequency spectrum, instead of concentrated in particular low frequency harmonic components, which allows it to be better attenuated by averaging or low-pass filtering.

Stochastic resonance is interesting because it is a seemingly paradoxical idea. Measurement systems are usually constructed or have evolved to reduce noise as much as possible and thereby provide the most precise measurement of the signal of interest. Numerous experiments show that in certain types of systems the addition of noise can actually improve the probability of detecting the signal; this is stochastic resonance. The systems in which stochastic resonance occur are always nonlinear systems; the addition of noise to a linear system will always decrease the SNR.

Signal Detection Perspective

Signal detection theory is a type of decision theory that uses probability distributions to explain a threshold decision mechanism. If stochastic resonance is present, the measurement of the ability to make the correct decision d' should be at a maximum with a particular level of noise present.

Stochastic Resonance in a Signal

Stochastic Resonance in an Image

The following demonstration is after Simonotto et al [2].

File:Daliface.png
Salvador Dali made use of this effect in "Gala contemplating the Mediterranean Sea which at twenty meters becomes a portrait of Abraham Lincoln"
File:Dali4ed.png
The above image of Dali after it is modified by the addition of noise and put through a threshold operation; each panel shows a different level of noise variance, with the minimum in the top left and maximum in the bottom right. The top right frame shows the optimal level of noise for allowing recognition of the original image.

If an image is put through a threshold filter, it can be hard to discern the objects in the original image because of the reduced amount of information present. The addition of noise before the threshold operation can result in a more recognizable output. The image below shows four versions of the image after thresholding with different levels of noise variance; the image in the top right hand corner appears to have the optimal level of noise allowing the object to be recognized.

The quality of the image resulting from stochastic resonance can be improved further by squinting one's eyes or moving away from the image. This allows one's brain to average the pixel intensities over areas, which is in effect a low-pass filter. The resonance breaks up the harmonic distortion by spreading it across the spectrum, and the low-pass filter eliminates much of the noise that has been pushed into higher spatial frequencies.

A similar output could be achieved by examining various threshold levels, so in a sense the addition of noise creates a new effective threshold for the measurement device.

History of Stochastic Resonance

Stochastic resonance was first discovered in a study of the periodic recurrence of earth’s ice ages [3] [4] The theory developed out of an effort to understand how the earth's climate oscillates periodically between two relatively stable global temperature states. The conventional explanation was that variations in the eccentricity of earth's orbital path occurred with a period of 100,000 years and caused the average temperature to shift dramatically. The measured variation in the eccentricity had a relatively small amplitude compared to the dramatic temperature change, however, and stochastic resonance was developed to show that the weak eccentricity variation with added stochastic variation could cause the temperature to move in a nonlinear fashion between two stable dynamic states.

Theoretical Grounding in Model Neurons

Not long after, the phenomenon was demonstrated in a mathematical model of a single neuron using a dynamical systems approach [5]. This analysis tested a pure tonal input with broadband noise and calculated the SNR from the power spectrum of the model neuron’s spike output. The characteristic peak for a particular input noise was apparent.

Animal Physiology Experiments

Crayfish Mechanoreceptors

Evidence for stochastic resonance in a sensory system was first found in nerve signals from the mechanoreceptors located on the tail fan of the crayfish (Procambarus clarkii)[6]. An appendage from the tail fan was mechanically stimulated to trigger the cuticular hairs that the crayfish uses to detect pressure waves in water. The stimulus consisted of sinusoidal motion at 55.2 Hz with random Gaussian noise at varying levels of average intensity. Spikes along the nerve root of the terminal abdominal ganglion were recorded extracellularly for 11 cells and analyzed to determine the SNR.

Two separate measures of SNR were used. The first was based on the Fourier power spectrum of the spike time series response. The power spectra from the averaged spike data for three different noise intensities all show a clear peak at the 55.2 Hz component with different average levels of broadband noise. The low- and mid-level noise conditions also show a second harmonic component at about 110 Hz. The mid-level noise condition clearly shows a stronger component at the signal of interest than either low- or high-level noise, and the harmonic component is greatly reduced at mid-level noise and not present in the high-level noise. A standard measure of the SNR shows a clear peak at the mid-level noise condition.

The other measure used for SNR was based on the inter-spike interval histogram. A similar peak was found for mid-level noise, although it was not equal to that found using the power spectrum measurement.

These data support the claim that noise can enhance detection at the single neuron level but are not enough to establish that noise helps the crayfish detect weak signals in a natural setting.

Cricket Mechanoreceptors

A similar experiment was performed on the cricket (Acheta domestica), a close evolutionary relative of the crayfish [7]. The cercal system in the cricket senses pressure waves in the atmosphere utilizing tympanic membranes located on its forelegs. Sensory interneurons in terminal abdominal ganglion carry information about intensity and direction of pressure perturbations. Crickets were presented with signal and noise stimuli and intracellular recordings from cercal interneurons were made.

Two types of measurements of stochastic resonance were conducted. The first, like the crayfish experiment, consisted of a pure tone pressure signal at 23 Hz in a broadband noise background of varying intensities. A power spectrum analysis of the signals yielded maximum SNR for a noise intensity equal to 25 times the signal stimulus resulting in a maximum increase of 600% in SNR. 14 cells in 12 animals were tested, and all showed an increased SNR with noise.

The other measurement consisted of the rate of mutual information transfer between the nerve signal and a broadband stimulus combined with varying levels of broadband noise uncorrelated with the signal. The power spectrum SNR could not be calculated in the same manner as before because there were signal and noise components present at the same frequencies. Mutual information measures the degree to which one signal predicts another; independent signals carry no mutual information, while perfectly identical signals carry maximal mutual information. For varying low amplitudes of signal, stochastic resonance peaks were found in plots of mutual information transfer rate as a function of input noise with a maximum increase in information transfer rate of 150%. For stronger signal amplitudes that stimulated the interneurons in the presence of no noise, however, the addition of noise always decreased the mutual information transfer demonstrating that stochastic resonance only works in the presence of low-intensity signals. The information carried in each spike at different levels of input noise was also calculated. At the optimum level of noise, the cells were more likely to spike, resulting in spikes with more information and more precise temporal coherence with the stimulus.

Stochastic resonance is a possible cause of escape behavior in crickets to attacks from predators that cause pressure waves in the tested frequency range at very low amplitudes, like the wasp Liris niger. Similar effects have also been noted in cockroaches.

Rat mechanoreceptors

Another investigation of stochastic resonance in broadband (or, equivalently, aperiodic) signals was conducted by probing cutaneous mechanoreceptors in the rat [8]. A patch of skin from the thigh and its corresponding section of the saphenous nerve were removed, mounted on a test stand immersed in interstitial fluid. Slowly adapting type 1 (SA1) mechanoreceptors output signals in response to mechanical vibrations below 500 Hz.

The skin was mechanically stimulated with a broadband pressure signal with varying amounts of broadband noise using tbe up-and-down motion of a cylindrical probe. The intensity of the pressure signal was tested without noise and then set at a near sub-threshold intensity that would evoke 10 action potentials over a 60-second stimulation time. Several trials were then conducted with noise of increasing amplitude variance. Extracellular recordings were made of the mechanoreceptor response from the extracted nerve.

The encoding of the pressure stimulus in the neural signal was measured by the coherence of the stimulus and response. The coherence was found to be maximized by a particular level of input Gaussian noise, consist with the occurrence of stochastic resonance.

Paddlefish Electroreceptors

The paddlefish (Polyodon spathula) hunts plankton using thousands of tiny passive electroreceptors located on its extended snout, or rostrum. The paddlefish is able to detect electric fields that oscillate at 0.5-20 Hz, and large groups of plankton generate this type of signal.

Due to the small magnitude of the generated fields, plankton are usually caught by the paddlefish when they are within 40 mm of the fish’s rostrum. An experiment was performed to test the hunting ability of the paddlefish in environments with different levels of background noise[9]. It was found that the paddlefish had a wider distance range of successful strikes in an electrical background with a low level of noise than in the absence of noise. In other words, there was a peak noise level, implying effects of stochastic resonance.

In the absence of noise, the distribution of successful strikes has greater variance in the horizontal direction than in the vertical direction. With the optimal level of noise, the variance in the vertical direction increased relative to the horizontal direction and also shifted to a peak slightly below center, although the horizontal variance did not increase.

Another measure of the increase in accuracy due to the optimal noise background is the number of plankton captured per unit time. For four paddlefish tested, two showed no increase in capture rate, while the other two showed a 50% increase in capture rate.

Observations of the paddlefish hunting in the wild provide evidence that the background noise generated by plankton increase the paddlefish’s hunting abilities. The individual plankton generate particular signals on top of the background signals from a large group, and the paddlefish do not respond to only noise without individual signals. For these reasons, it is likely that the paddlefish takes advantage of stochastic resonance to improve its sensitivity to prey.

Multi-Unit Systems of Model Neurons

An aspect of stochastic resonance that is not entirely understood has to do with the relative magnitude of stimuli and the threshold for triggering the sensory neurons that measure them. If the stimuli are generally of a certain magnitude, it seems that it would be more evolutionarily advantageous for the threshold of the neuron to match that of the stimuli. In systems with noise, however, tuning thresholds for taking advantage of stochastic resonance may be the best strategy.

A theoretical account of how a large model network (up to 1000) of summing FitzHugh-Nagumo neurons could adjust the threshold of the system based on the noise level present in the environment was devised [10]. This can be equivalently conceived of as the system lowering its threshold, and this is accomplished such that the ability to detect suprathreshold signals is not degraded.

Stochastic resonance in physiological systems of neurons have not yet been found experimentally.

Stochastic Resonance in Human Perception

Psychophysical experiments testing the thresholds of sensory systems have also been performed in humans across sensory modalities and have yielded evidence that our systems make use of stochastic resonance as well.

Vision

The above demonstration using the Dali photo is a simplified version of an earlier experiment[2]. In that case, the noise input to the image varied over time, and the subjects perceived that the image was most recognizable at a certain level of noise.

Tactile

Human subjects who undergo mechanical stimulation of a fingertip are able to detect a subthreshold impulse signal in the presence of a noisy mechanical vibration. The percentage of correct detections of the presence of the signal was maximized for a particular value of noise. [11]

Audition

The auditory intensity detection thresholds of a number of human subjects were tested in the presence of noise [12]. The subjects include four people with normal hearing, two with cochlear implants and one with an auditory-brainstem implant. The normal subjects were presented with two sound samples, one with a pure tone plus white noise and one with just white noise, and asked which one contained the pure tone. The level of noise which optimized the detection threshold in all four subjects was found to be between -15 and -20 dB relative to the pure tone.

A similar test in the subjects with cochlear implants only found improved detection thresholds for pure tones below 300 Hz, while improvements were found at frequencies greater than 60 Hz in the brainstem implant subject. The reason for the limited range of resonance effects are unknown. Additionally, the addition of noise to cochlear implant signals improved the threshold for frequency discrimination. The authors recommend that some type of white noise addition to cochlear implant signals could well improve the utility of such devices.

Gamma Oscillations

References

  1. ^ [1] Moss, F., Ward, L.M., Sannita, W.G. "Stochastic resonance and sensory information processing: a tutorial and review of application" Clinical Neurophysiology, Volume 115, Issue 2, February 2004, Pages 267-281. (2004).
  2. ^ a b [2] Simonotto, E., Riani, M., Seife, C., Roberts, M., Twitty, J., Moss, F. “Visual perception of stochastic resonance” Physical Review Letters. (1997).
  3. ^ [3] Benzi, R., Sutera, A., Vulpiani, A. “The mechanism of statistic resonance”, Journal of Physics A, Vol. 14. (1981).
  4. ^ [4] Benzi, R., Parisi, G., Sutera, A., Vulpiani, A. “A theory of stochastic resonance in climatic change”, SIAM Journal on Applied Mathematics, 1983.
  5. ^ [5] Bulsara, A., Jacobs, E.W., Zhou, T., Moss, F., Kiss, L. “Stochastic resonance in a single neuron model: theory and analog simulation”, Journal of Theoretical Biology (1991).
  6. ^ [6] Douglass, J., Wilkens, L. Pantazelou, E. and Moss, F., Noise enhancement of information transfer in crayfish mechanoreceptors by stochastic resonance. Nature 365, 337-340. (1993).
  7. ^ [7] Levin, J.E. and Miller, J.P. Broadband neural encoding in the cricket cercal sensory system enhanced by stochastic resonance. Nature 380, 165-168 (1996).
  8. ^ [8] Collins, J.J., Imhoff, T.T., Grigg, P. "Transmission in rat SA1 cutaneous mechanoreceptors via aperiodic stochastic resonance". Journal of Neurophysiology. (1996).
  9. ^ [9] Russell, D.F., Wilkens, L.A., Moss, F. "Use of behavioural stochastic resonance by paddle fish for feeding", Nature 402, 291-294. (1999).
  10. ^ [10] Collins, J.J., Chow, C.C., Imhoff, T.T. “Stochastic resonance without tuning”, Nature. (1995).
  11. ^ [11] Collins, J.J., Imhoff, T.T., Griggs, P. “Noise enhanced Tactile Sensation”, Nature, Vol. 383. (1996).
  12. ^ Zeng, F.G., Fu, Q.J., Morse, R. “Human hearing enhanced by noise [12]”. Brain Research. (2000).
  • Peter Hanggi. "Stochastic Resonance in Biology", Chemphyschem 3, 285-290. (2002).
  • Wiesenfeld, K., Moss, F. "Stochastic resonance and the benefits of noise: from ice ages to crayfish and SQUIDs". Nature. (1995).
  • Moss, F., Wilkens, L.A. "Mechanoreceptors and Stochastic resonance [13]", Scholarpedia, 2008.