Jump to content

Race and intelligence: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Test bias: the debate on this is still open
reverted to Bainer version. On eveluation too much was changed too quickly for us to see what he did and evaluate it
Line 1: Line 1:
{{POV}}
{{POV}}
{{Cleanup|date=October 2007}}
{{Cleanup|date=October 2007}}
{{Race}}
{{Race}}

The study of '''race and intelligence''' is the controversial study of how human intellectual capacities may vary among the different population groups commonly known as [[race]]s. This study seeks to identify and explain the differences in manifestations of intelligence (e.g. [[IQ]] testing results), as well as the underlying causes of such variance.
The study of '''race and intelligence''' is the controversial study of how human intellectual capacities may vary among the different population groups commonly known as [[race]]s. This study seeks to identify and explain the differences in manifestations of intelligence (e.g. [[IQ]] testing results), as well as the underlying causes of such variance.


Line 35: Line 36:


[[Image:Francis Galton 1850s.jpg|right|thumb|140px|Sir [[Francis Galton]] wrote on [[eugenics]] and [[psychometrics]] in the 19th C.]]
[[Image:Francis Galton 1850s.jpg|right|thumb|140px|Sir [[Francis Galton]] wrote on [[eugenics]] and [[psychometrics]] in the 19th C.]]
[[Image:FranzBoas.jpg|thumb|140px|right|[[Anthropology|Anthropologist]] [[Franz Boas]] was a prominent 20th C. critic of claims that intelligence differed among races.]]

[[Image:Ruth Benedict.jpg|thumb|right|140px|[[Ruth Benedict]] was an anthropologist who challenged the idea that people of different races had different inherent intelligences.]]
[[Image:Ruth Benedict.jpg|thumb|right|140px|[[Ruth Benedict]] was an anthropologist who challenged the idea that people of different races had different inherent intelligences.]]


Because the [[Atlantic slave trade]] raised moral questions from its inception, scientific theories about the mental capacities of Black people were provided to justify the enslavement of Africans. According to Alexander Thomas and Samuell Sillen, during this time period the Black man was described as uniquely fitted for bondage because of what researchers at the time called "his primitive psychological organization."<ref>[[Alexander Thomas]] and [[Samuell Sillen]] (1972).'' Racism and Psychiatry''. New York: Carol Publishing Group. </ref> Hence, a well-known physician of the antebellum South, Samuel Cartwright of Louisiana, had a psychiatric explanation for runaway slaves. He diagnosed their attempts to gain freedom as a mental illness and coined the term "[[drapetomania]]" to describe it.<ref>Samual A. Cartwright, [http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/aia/part4/4h3106t.html "Diseases and Peculiarities of the Negro Race"], ''[[DeBow's Review]]—Southern and Western States'', Volume XI, New Orleans, 1851</ref> The writings of Sir [[Francis Galton]], a British psychologist, spurred interest in the study of mental abilities, particularly as they relate to [[heredity]] and [[eugenics]].<ref>[http://iml.jou.ufl.edu/projects/Spring02/Holland/Galton.htm Eugenics: America's Darkest Days]</ref> Galton estimated from his field observations in Africa that the African people were "two grades" below Anglo-Saxons' position in the normal frequency distribution of general mental ability.<ref>[http://www.indiana.edu/~intell/galton.shtml Francis Galton:British Psychologist]</ref>
Because the [[Atlantic slave trade]] raised moral questions from its inception, scientific theories about the mental capacities of Black people were provided to justify the enslavement of Africans. According to Alexander Thomas and Samuell Sillen, during this time period the Black man was described as uniquely fitted for bondage because of what researchers at the time called "his primitive psychological organization."<ref>[[Alexander Thomas]] and [[Samuell Sillen]] (1972).'' Racism and Psychiatry''. New York: Carol Publishing Group. </ref> Hence, a well-known physician of the antebellum South, Samuel Cartwright of Louisiana, had a psychiatric explanation for runaway slaves. He diagnosed their attempts to gain freedom as a mental illness and coined the term "[[drapetomania]]" to describe it.<ref>Samual A. Cartwright, [http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/aia/part4/4h3106t.html "Diseases and Peculiarities of the Negro Race"], ''[[DeBow's Review]]—Southern and Western States'', Volume XI, New Orleans, 1851</ref>


Scientific arguments about the mental inferiority of Black people were instrumental in keeping slavery alive as an institution in the United States. It was widely regarded that Black people lacked the mental capacity to handle freedom. [[Secretary of State]] [[John C. Calhoun]] arguing for the extension of slavery in 1844 said, "Here (scientific confirmation) is proof of the necessity of slavery. The African is incapable of self-care and sinks into lunacy under the burden of freedom. It is a mercy to give him the guardianship and protection from mental death."
Scientific arguments about the mental inferiority of Black people were instrumental in keeping slavery alive as an institution in the United States. It was widely regarded that Black people lacked the mental capacity to handle freedom. [[Secretary of State]] [[John C. Calhoun]] arguing for the extension of slavery in 1844 said, "Here (scientific confirmation) is proof of the necessity of slavery. The African is incapable of self-care and sinks into lunacy under the burden of freedom. It is a mercy to give him the guardianship and protection from mental death."

The writings of Sir [[Francis Galton]], a British psychologist, spurred interest in the study of mental abilities, particularly as they relate to [[heredity]] and [[eugenics]].<ref>[http://iml.jou.ufl.edu/projects/Spring02/Holland/Galton.htm Eugenics: America's Darkest Days]</ref> Galton estimated from his field observations in Africa that the African people were "two grades" below Anglo-Saxons' position in the normal frequency distribution of general mental ability. His work was seen as scientific validation of Africans' mental inferiority compared with Anglo-Saxons.<ref>[http://www.indiana.edu/~intell/galton.shtml Francis Galton:British Psychologist]</ref>


====Immigration and segregation====
====Immigration and segregation====


In the 19th and 20th centuries research on race and intelligence has still been used to argue that one race is superior to another, justifying poor outcomes and treatment for the "inferior race".<ref>''[[Social Darwinism]], [[Scientific racism|Scientific Racism]], and the Metaphysics of Race'' Rutledge M. Dennis The Journal of Negro Education, Vol. 64, No. 3, Myths and Realities: African Americans and the Measurement of Human Abilities (Summer, 1995), pp. 243-252</ref> Researchers such as Amanda Thompson and Elazar Barkan have suggested that "[[Scientific racism]]" has been used to perpetuate the idea of the intellectual inferiority of [[African Americans]] and that it was used to justify segregated education in America.
In the 19th and 20th centuries research on race and intelligence has still been used to argue that one race is superior to another, justifying poor outcomes and treatment for the "inferior race".<ref>''[[Social Darwinism]], [[Scientific racism|Scientific Racism]], and the Metaphysics of Race'' Rutledge M. Dennis The Journal of Negro Education, Vol. 64, No. 3, Myths and Realities: African Americans and the Measurement of Human Abilities (Summer, 1995), pp. 243-252</ref> Researchers such as Amanda Thompson and Elazar Barkan have suggested that "[[Scientific racism]]" has been used to perpetuate the idea of the intellectual inferiority of [[African Americans]] and that it was used to justify segregated education in America.

The scientific debate on the contribution of [[nature versus nurture]] to individual and group differences in intelligence can be traced to at least the mid-19th century.<ref>{{AYref|Degler|1992}}; {{AYref|Loehlin et al.|1975}}</ref> [[Charles Darwin]] wrote in his ''[[Descent of Man]]'' (VII, ''On the races of Man''):
"Their mental characteristics are likewise very distinct; chiefly as it would appear in their emotional, but partly in their intellectual faculties."


Lewis Terman wrote in ''The measurement of intelligence'' in 1916 <blockquote>"(Black and other ethnic minority children) are uneducable beyond the nearest rudiments of training. No amount of school instruction will ever make them intelligent voters or capable citizens in the sense of the world…their dullness seems to be racial, or at least inherent in the family stock from which they come…Children of this group should be segregated in special classes and be given instruction which is concrete and practical. They cannot master abstractions, but they can be made efficient workers…There is no possibility at present of convincing society that they should not be allowed to reproduce, although from a eugenic point of view they constitute a grave problem because of their unusual prolific breeding."</blockquote>
Lewis Terman wrote in ''The measurement of intelligence'' in 1916 <blockquote>"(Black and other ethnic minority children) are uneducable beyond the nearest rudiments of training. No amount of school instruction will ever make them intelligent voters or capable citizens in the sense of the world…their dullness seems to be racial, or at least inherent in the family stock from which they come…Children of this group should be segregated in special classes and be given instruction which is concrete and practical. They cannot master abstractions, but they can be made efficient workers…There is no possibility at present of convincing society that they should not be allowed to reproduce, although from a eugenic point of view they constitute a grave problem because of their unusual prolific breeding."</blockquote>


The opinion that there are differences in the brain sizes and brain structures of different racial and ethnic groups was widely held and studied during the 19th century and early 20th century.<ref>{{AYref|Broca|1873}}, {{AYref|Bean|1906}}, {{AYref|Mall|1909}}, {{AYref|Morton|1839}}, {{AYref|Pearl|1934}}, {{AYref|Vint|1934}}</ref> Average ethnic and racial group differences in IQ were observed when analyzing the data from standardized mental tests administered on large scales during [[World War I]]. For example, in this test "Southern Whites", scored below "Northern Negroes."<ref>''Outcome-Based Tyranny: Teaching Compliance While Testing Like A State'' IQ tests administered to the American Expeditionary Forces (AEF) in World War I. Anthropological Quarterly - Volume 76, Number 4, Fall 2003, pp. 715-730</ref> These results inspired the first theories of environmental influences on intelligence. An early advocate of these ideas was [[Ruth Benedict]], who in her book, ''The Races of Mankind'' challenged the idea that people of different races had different inherent intelligences.<blockquote>''The difference arose because of differences of income, education, cultural advantages, and other opportunities.'' --Ruth Benedict</blockquote>
The opinion that there are differences in the brain sizes and brain structures of different racial and ethnic groups was widely held and studied during the 19th century and early 20th century.<ref>{{AYref|Broca|1873}}, {{AYref|Bean|1906}}, {{AYref|Mall|1909}}, {{AYref|Morton|1839}}, {{AYref|Pearl|1934}}, {{AYref|Vint|1934}}</ref> Average ethnic and racial group differences in IQ were observed when analyzing the data from standardized mental tests administered on large scales during [[World War I]]. For example, in this test "Southern Whites", scored below "Northern Negroes."<ref>''Outcome-Based Tyranny: Teaching Compliance While Testing Like A State'' IQ tests administered to the American Expeditionary Forces (AEF) in World War I. Anthropological Quarterly - Volume 76, Number 4, Fall 2003, pp. 715-730</ref> These results inspired the first theories of environmental influences on intelligence. An early advocate of these ideas was [[Ruth Benedict]], who in her book, ''The Races of Mankind'' challenged the idea that people of different races had different inherent intelligences.<blockquote>''The difference arose because of differences of income, education, cultural advantages, and other opportunities.'' --Ruth Benedict</blockquote>

Foremost amongst those researching this was [[Stanley Porteus]], who although not a staff member, gave some lectures at the [[University of Melbourne]], devised his [[Porteus Maze Test|maze test]] as early as 1913, later applying it in his study of the [[Indigenous Australians|Aborigines]] in the [[Kimberley region of Western Australia|Kimberley]] region and [[Northern Territory]] of Australia (1929) and later the [[Kalahari]] tribesmen of southern Africa (1934). He also used it to assess the results of pre-frontal brain surgery on mental performance, publishing his results in 1931.<ref>Porteus,
Stanley. ''The Psychology of a Primitive People'', 1931.</ref>

W.O. Brown, writing in ''The Journal of Negro History'' in 1931, wrote regarding early intelligence tests:

<blockquote>''After the World War and during the severe agitation for the restriction of immigration, aimed especially at the Southeastern Europeans, tests came into a new usage. ..the tests revealed the inferior intelligence of various racial and nationality groups. ..The Southeastern Europeans and the Negroes especially came of badly in these tests. ..The results of the tests elevated their dogma of racial inequality from a mere prejudice to the dignity of a scientifically validated opinion.''<ref>[http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0022-2992%28193101%2916%3A1%3C43%3ARIFOM%3E2.0.CO%3B2-E Racial Inequality: Fact or Myth] W. O. Brown, The Journal of Negro History, Vol. 16, No. 1. (Jan., 1931), pp. 49</ref></blockquote>


Dorthy Roberts writes that the history of the eugenics movement in America was strongly tied to the older scientific racism used to justify slavery. Eugenicists claimed that the IQ test could quantify innate human ability in a single measurement, despite the objections of the creator of the test, [[Alfred Binet]].<ref>''Killing the Black Body: Race, Reproduction, and the Meaning of Liberty'' by Dorothy Roberts. Page 63. December 1998 ISBN 0679758690</ref> Beginning in the 1930s, race difference research and [[hereditarianism]] — the belief that [[genetics]] are the primary cause of differences in intelligence among human groups — began to fall out of favor in psychology and anthropology after major internal debates.<ref>According to historian of psychology Graham Richards there was widespread critical debate within psychology about the conceptual underpinnings of this early race difference research ({{AYref|Richards|1997}}). These include Estabrooks (1928) two papers on the limitations of methodology used in the research; Dearborn and Long’s (1934) overview of the criticisms by several psychologists (Garth, Thompson, Peterson, Pinter, Herskovits, Daniel, Price, Wilkerson, Freeman, Rosenthal and C.E. Smith) in a collection they edited and Klineburg, who wrote three major critiques, one in 1928, and two in 1935. Richards also notes that with over a 1000 publications within psychology during the interwar years there had been a large internal debate. Towards the end of the time period almost all those publishing, including most of those who began with a pro-race differences stance, were firmly arguing against race differences research. Richards regards the scientific controversy to be dead at this point, although he also suggests reasons for its re-emergence in the late nineteen sixties.</ref> In anthropology this occurred in part due to the advocacy of [[Franz Boas]], who in his 1938 edition of ''The Mind of Primitive Man'' wrote, "there is nothing at all that could be interpreted as suggesting any material difference in the mental capacity of the bulk of the Negro population as compared with the bulk of the White population."<ref>{{AYref|Boas|1938}}</ref>
Dorthy Roberts writes that the history of the eugenics movement in America was strongly tied to the older scientific racism used to justify slavery. Eugenicists claimed that the IQ test could quantify innate human ability in a single measurement, despite the objections of the creator of the test, [[Alfred Binet]].<ref>''Killing the Black Body: Race, Reproduction, and the Meaning of Liberty'' by Dorothy Roberts. Page 63. December 1998 ISBN 0679758690</ref> Beginning in the 1930s, race difference research and [[hereditarianism]] — the belief that [[genetics]] are the primary cause of differences in intelligence among human groups — began to fall out of favor in psychology and anthropology after major internal debates.<ref>According to historian of psychology Graham Richards there was widespread critical debate within psychology about the conceptual underpinnings of this early race difference research ({{AYref|Richards|1997}}). These include Estabrooks (1928) two papers on the limitations of methodology used in the research; Dearborn and Long’s (1934) overview of the criticisms by several psychologists (Garth, Thompson, Peterson, Pinter, Herskovits, Daniel, Price, Wilkerson, Freeman, Rosenthal and C.E. Smith) in a collection they edited and Klineburg, who wrote three major critiques, one in 1928, and two in 1935. Richards also notes that with over a 1000 publications within psychology during the interwar years there had been a large internal debate. Towards the end of the time period almost all those publishing, including most of those who began with a pro-race differences stance, were firmly arguing against race differences research. Richards regards the scientific controversy to be dead at this point, although he also suggests reasons for its re-emergence in the late nineteen sixties.</ref> In anthropology this occurred in part due to the advocacy of [[Franz Boas]], who in his 1938 edition of ''The Mind of Primitive Man'' wrote, "there is nothing at all that could be interpreted as suggesting any material difference in the mental capacity of the bulk of the Negro population as compared with the bulk of the White population."<ref>{{AYref|Boas|1938}}</ref>


Inspired by the American eugenics movement, [[Nazi Germany]] implemented the [[T-4 Euthanasia Program]] in which roughly 200,000 mentally and physically disabled Germans were killed, and about 400,000 sterilized. The association of hereditarianism with [[Nazi Germany]] created a modern academic environment that has been very skeptical of suggestions that there are racial or ethnic differences in measures of intellectual or academic ability and that these differences are primarily determined by genetic factors.<ref>{{AYref|Garrett|1961}}; {{AYref|Lynn|2001}}, pp. 45–54</ref>
Inspired by the American eugenics movement, [[Nazi Germany]] implemented the [[T-4 Euthanasia Program]] in which roughly 200,000 mentally and physically disabled Germans were killed, and about 400,000 sterilized. The association of hereditarianism with [[Nazi Germany]] created a modern academic environment that has been very skeptical of suggestions that there are racial or ethnic differences in measures of intellectual or academic ability and that these differences are primarily determined by genetic factors.<ref>{{AYref|Garrett|1961}}; {{AYref|Lynn|2001}}, pp. 45–54</ref>
Line 80: Line 93:


In the official statements of position endorsed by the [[American Sociological Association]] and the [[American Anthropological Association]],<ref>[http://www.aaanet.org/stmts/race.htm Statement on "Race" and Intelligence] American Anthropological Association</ref> as reported in ''The New York Times'',<ref name="NYT Race">[http://www.nytimes.com/2003/03/20/health/20RACE.html ''2 Scholarly Articles Diverge On Role of Race in Medicine''] By NICHOLAS WADE Published: [[March 20]], [[2003]]] New York Times</ref>
In the official statements of position endorsed by the [[American Sociological Association]] and the [[American Anthropological Association]],<ref>[http://www.aaanet.org/stmts/race.htm Statement on "Race" and Intelligence] American Anthropological Association</ref> as reported in ''The New York Times'',<ref name="NYT Race">[http://www.nytimes.com/2003/03/20/health/20RACE.html ''2 Scholarly Articles Diverge On Role of Race in Medicine''] By NICHOLAS WADE Published: [[March 20]], [[2003]]] New York Times</ref>
"A view widespread among many social scientists is that race is not a valid biological concept. However scientific reporter [[Nicholas Wade]], writing in the New York Times said that: '"biologists, particularly the population geneticists who study genetic variation, have found that there is a race structure in the human population; a family tree showing separate branches for Africans, Caucasians (Europe, the Middle East, and the Indian subcontinent), East Asians, Pacific Islanders, and American Indians."<ref name="NYT Race" />
"A view widespread among many social scientists is that race is not a valid biological concept. However, biologists, particularly the population geneticists who study genetic variation, have found that there is a race structure in the human population; a family tree showing separate branches for Africans, Caucasians (Europe, the Middle East, and the Indian subcontinent), East Asians, Pacific Islanders, and American Indians."<ref name="NYT Race" />

Proponents of partly-genetic explanations of race/IQ correlation have often been criticized because much of their work is funded by the [[Pioneer Fund]].({{AYref|Tucker|2002}}) The Pioneer Fund has been characterized by the [[Southern Poverty Law Center]] as a [[hate group]]. Conversely, supporters of race and intelligence research have accused other scientists of [[scientific misconduct|suppressing scientific debate]] for political purposes. They claim harassment and interference with the work or funding of partly-genetic proponents.<ref>See for example Morton Hunt's ''The New Know-Nothings: The Political Foes of the Scientific Study of Human Nature'' (1999; pp. 63-104) which argues that recent years "have witnessed a dramatic upsurge in efforts to impose limits on the freedom of social scientists to explore controversial research questions, particularly questions that could yield answers distasteful to those with certain sociopolitical or ideological agendas" ({{AYref|Lilienfeld|2002}}).</ref>

A racist motivation is frequently ascribed to some researchers who work on questions of race and intelligence. Both historical and contemporary researchers have been described as [[racism|racists]],<ref>Gould, 1981</ref> and some critics hold that it is racist to assert that there are cognitive or behavioral differences between ethnic groups. For example, psychologist Jerry Hirsch has claimed that Arthur Jensen has "avowed goals" that were "as heinously barbaric as were Hitler's and the anti-abolitionists"<ref>Hunt, M. M. (1998). ''The New Know-Nothings: The Political Foes of the Scientific Study of Human Nature.'' New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction. ISBN 1-56000-393-6.</ref>


===Race===
===Race===
Line 107: Line 116:
{{Main|intelligence quotient}}
{{Main|intelligence quotient}}


All such tests are often called "intelligence tests," though the use of the term "intelligence" is itself controversial. A low but significant correlation was found in tests administered to two groups of kindergarten children in a study reported in 1991<ref> J Clin Psychol. 1991 Sep;47(5):698-702.</ref><ref>[http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=1939717&dopt=Citation Predictive validity of two short-forms of the WPPSI: a 3-year follow-up study.]</ref> School grades are the better predicator of later academic success than IQ and the relations may be lower for specific populations. In a sample of 127 students enrolled in a private day school located in a large metropolitan area, the correlations ranged from .11 to .22 with the median of .18.<ref>''The Predictive Value of IQ'' Sternberg, Robert J. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly - Volume 47, Number 1, January 2001, pp. 1-41</ref> Nevertheless, the predictive value of IQ at predicting later academic success in children do not disprove per se its capacity of measuring intelligence.
All such tests are often called "intelligence tests," though the use of the term "intelligence" is itself controversial. A low but significant correlation was found in tests administered to two groups of kindergarten children in a study reported in 1991<ref> J Clin Psychol. 1991 Sep;47(5):698-702.</ref><ref>[http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=1939717&dopt=Citation Predictive validity of two short-forms of the WPPSI: a 3-year follow-up study.]</ref> School grades are the better predicator of later academic success than IQ and the relations may be lower for specific populations. In a sample of 127 students enrolled in a private day school located in a large metropolitan area, the correlations ranged from .11 to .22 with the median of .18.<ref>''The Predictive Value of IQ'' Sternberg, Robert J. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly - Volume 47, Number 1, January 2001, pp. 1-41</ref>


"Many of the most widely used tests are not intended to measure intelligence itself but some closely related construct: scholastic aptitude, school achievement, specific abilities... . Scores on intelligence-related tests matter, and the stakes can be high," according to the task force appointed by the Board of Scientific Affairs of the [[American Psychological Association]]. Such tests are argued to be good measures of the [[psychometric]] variable '''g''' (for [[general intelligence factor|general intelligence factor'']]). While some psychologists regard '''g''' as the fundamental measure of intelligence, others emphasize the strengths and weaknesses present in each person's performance on different aspects of the tests.<ref>[http://www.gifted.uconn.edu/siegle/research/Correlation/Intelligence.pdf Text of the APA Task Force Report, "Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns"]</ref>
"Many of the most widely used tests are not intended to measure intelligence itself but some closely related construct: scholastic aptitude, school achievement, specific abilities... . Scores on intelligence-related tests matter, and the stakes can be high," according to the task force appointed by the Board of Scientific Affairs of the [[American Psychological Association]]. Such tests are argued to be good measures of the [[psychometric]] variable '''g''' (for [[general intelligence factor|general intelligence factor'']]). While some psychologists regard '''g''' as the fundamental measure of intelligence, others emphasize the strengths and weaknesses present in each person's performance on different aspects of the tests.<ref>[http://www.gifted.uconn.edu/siegle/research/Correlation/Intelligence.pdf Text of the APA Task Force Report, "Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns"]</ref>
Line 123: Line 132:
Another theory is the [[Triarchic theory of intelligence]] which was formulated by [[Robert J. Sternberg]]. According to this theory the three components of intelligence are analytic intelligence, creative intelligence, and practical intelligence. According to Sternberg, only analytic intelligence is measured by standardized IQ tests.
Another theory is the [[Triarchic theory of intelligence]] which was formulated by [[Robert J. Sternberg]]. According to this theory the three components of intelligence are analytic intelligence, creative intelligence, and practical intelligence. According to Sternberg, only analytic intelligence is measured by standardized IQ tests.


====Environment====

[[Steve Sailer]] has stated that,
<blockquote>
"a bad environment can hurt IQ and can be seen in the IQ scores for sub-Saharan African countries. They average only around 70. In contrast, African-Americans average about 85. It appears unlikely that African-Americans’ white admixture can account for most of this 15-point gap because they are only around 17%-18% white on average, according to the latest genetic research. (Thus African-Americans white genes probably couldn't account for more than 3 points of the gap between African-Americans and Africans.) This suggests that the harshness of life in Africa might be cutting ten points or more off African IQ scores."[http://www.vdare.com/Sailer/wealth_of_nations.htm]
</blockquote>


== Research ==
== Research ==
{{main|Race and intelligence (Research)}}

=== Test data===
=== Test data===
{{main|Race and intelligence (test data)}}
The gaps found between the average intelligences of races or ethnicities varies depending on methods used for racial grouping, the method and setting used to test intelligence,<ref>Carraher, Carraher, and Schliemann (1985) studied a group of Brazilian street children. The investigation found that the same children who are able to do the mathematics needed to run their street businesses were often unable to do mathematics in a formal setting. See: ''Street Mathematics and School Mathematics'' By Terezinha Nunes, David William Carraher, Analucia Dias Schliemann ISBN 0521388139</ref> the health and economic situation of the test takers, the interplay between the culture of the person taking the test and the culture of those who made the test, and the period in history when the test was performed.
The gaps found between the average intelligences of races or ethnicities varies depending on methods used for racial grouping, the method and setting used to test intelligence,<ref>Carraher, Carraher, and Schliemann (1985) studied a group of Brazilian street children. The investigation found that the same children who are able to do the mathematics needed to run their street businesses were often unable to do mathematics in a formal setting. See: ''Street Mathematics and School Mathematics'' By Terezinha Nunes, David William Carraher, Analucia Dias Schliemann ISBN 0521388139</ref> the health and economic situation of the test takers, the interplay between the culture of the person taking the test and the culture of those who made the test, and the period in history when the test was performed.


Depending on the way intelligence is measured a variety of gaps may be found between different racial and ethnic groups. Some groups that perform well on one task may do poorly on others.<ref>''Mind in Context: Interactionist Perspectives on Human Intelligence'' By [[Robert J. Sternberg]], [[Richard K. Wagner]]</ref><ref>''Standardization of the Panga Munthu Test-A Nonverbal Cognitive Test Developed in Zambia Ravinder Kathuria'', [[Robert Serpell]] The Journal of Negro Education, Vol. 67, No. 3, Assessment in the Context of Culture and Pedagogy (Summer, 1998), pp. 228-241</ref>
Depending on the way intelligence is measured a variety of gaps may be found between different racial and ethnic groups. Some groups that perform well on one task may do poorly on others.<ref>''Mind in Context: Interactionist Perspectives on Human Intelligence'' By [[Robert J. Sternberg]], [[Richard K. Wagner]]</ref><ref>''Standardization of the Panga Munthu Test-A Nonverbal Cognitive Test Developed in Zambia Ravinder Kathuria'', [[Robert Serpell]] The Journal of Negro Education, Vol. 67, No. 3, Assessment in the Context of Culture and Pedagogy (Summer, 1998), pp. 228-241</ref>


Attempted world-wide compilations by Herrnstein and Murray, authors of ''The Bell Curve'', [[Richard Lynn]] and Rushton of average IQ by race generally place [[Ashkenazi Jews]] and [[East Asians]] at the top, followed by [[White (people)|Whites]], [[Arabs]] and [[Indigenous peoples of the Americas|Native Americans]], [[sub-Saharan Africa]]ns and [[Australian Aboriginal]]s.<ref>{{AYref|Herrnstein and Murray|1994}}; {{AYref|Lynn|1991a}}; {{AYref|Lynn|2006}}</ref><ref name="Rushton-review">{{cite journal | author = Rushton, J. P. | year = 2006 | month = | title = Lynn Richard, Race Differences in Intelligence: An Evolutionary Analysis, Washington Summit Books, Augusta, GA (2005) ISBN 1-59368-020-1, 318 pages., US$34.95 | journal = Personality and Individual Differences | volume = 40 | issue = 4 | pages = 853-855 | doi = 10.1016/j.paid.2005.10.004 | url = }}</ref> <ref name="main">Lynn, R. and Vanhanen, T. (2002). IQ and the wealth of nations. Westport, CT: Praeger. ISBN 0-275-97510-X</ref>
In the past 20 years the use of the IQ test as the sole measure of intelligence and the ability of intelligence tests to predict intelligence between people of different cultural backgrounds has fallen under increasing criticism. “Perhaps the best way to achieve coherence in the field of intelligence is to recognize that no single correct “model” or “approach” is evident and that different ones elucidate different aspects of a very complex phenomenon (Sternberg, 2003).”


The IQ scores vary greatly among different nations for the same group. Blacks in Africa score much lower than Blacks in the US. However contrary to indications from the [[IQ and the Wealth of Nations]] study, the majority of blacks enrolled in [[Ivy League]] Universities in the US are either from Africa or the Caribbean. The chairperson of the sociology department at Harvard University stated: "''Since they come from majority-black countries, they are less psychologically handicapped by the stigma of race.''" This is seen as evidence that racial prejudice combined with the status of being a minority that has been excluded from society does have a significant effect on academic achievement.<ref>[http://www.uh.edu/ednews/2004/nytimes/200406/20040624harvard.html Top Colleges Take More Blacks, but Which Ones?]</ref><ref>[http://www.sfgate.com/cgi-bin/article.cgi?f=/c/a/2005/02/22/MNGIJBF3LP1.DTL Shades of gray in black enrollment]</ref> However, according to the African-American economist [[Thomas Sowell]] racism and the legacy of slavery do not stand up under scrutiny of historical facts as explanations to the IQ disparity between Blacks and Whites. He argues that "redneck" black culture is the reason for the low IQ and poor academic performance of black Americans.<ref>[http://www.opinionjournal.com/editorial/feature.html?id=110006608 Crippled by Their Culture] OpinionJournal, WSJ.com </ref> Gaps are seen in other tests of cognitive ability or aptitude, including university admission exams such as the [[SAT]] and [[GRE]] as well as employment tests for corporate settings and the military (Roth et al. 2001).
==== IQ test score gap in the US ====
In the United States, the mean IQ score among [[Black (people)|Black]]s has at times been measured as approximately 85 and the mean IQ score among [[White (people)|White]]s has at times has been measured as approximately 100;<ref name="IQconsensus">For consensus statements see {{AYref|Gottfredson|1997a}} and {{AYref|Neisser et al.|1996}}.</ref> the mean IQ score of Latinos has been reported to be measured as approximately 89 for unspecified dates.<ref>Herrnstein and Murray in ''[[The Bell Curve]]''</ref>


Another researcher [[Philippe Rushton]] found African university students averaged an IQ of 84. In some studies, by other researchers, they have scored lower (IQ = 77). In still others of our studies, highly-selected engineering students who took math and science courses in high school scored higher (IQ = 103). Rushton also points out that immigrants regardless of race outperform native populations and adds that in theory Africans would revert back to their normal IQ in future generations if kept in the harsh African environment and cultural setting.<ref>http://www.vdare.com/misc/rushton_african_iq.htm</ref>
The mean score for people of [[East Asian]] and Jewish descent is usually higher than the mean score of Whites. However, several studies place the median IQ of [[Ashkenazi]] Jews (who make up the overwhelming majority of [[American Jews]]{{Fact|date=February 2007}}) at approximately one standard deviation above the mean for other Whites, with the primary Jewish advantage in verbal reasoning and the East Asian advantage primarily in spatial reasoning. In ''The Bell Curve'', Herrnstein and Murray report mean IQ scores for East Asians and Jewish Americans of 106 and 113, respectively (on a scale where Whites = 100).

[[Facial recognition]] ability has also shown differences by race.<ref>[http://www.apa.org/releases/facerecog.html PEOPLE ARE POOR AT CROSS-RACE FACIAL] APA News Release December 3, 2000</ref> Richard Ferraro writes that facial recognition is an example of a neuropsychological measure that can be used to assess cognitive abilities that are salient within African-American culture.<ref>''Minority and Cross-Cultural Aspects of Neuropsychological Assessment'' By F. Richard Ferraro Page 90 ISBN 9026518307</ref> In the US Blacks' performance is significantly better than that of whites', and blacks are better at recognizing faces of whites than whites are at recognizing blacks.<ref>''Children's Ability to Recognize Other Children's Faces'' Saul Feinman, Doris R. Entwisle ''Child Development, Vol. 47, No. 2'' (Jun., 1976), pp. 506-510</ref> A 1991 study found that white subjects performed significantly more poorly on trials involving African American faces than on trials involving White faces, whereas no such difference was obtained among African American subjects.<ref name="otherrace">''[http://web.uvic.ca/~slindsay/publications/1991LindJackChristian.pdf Other-Race Face Perception]'' D. Stephen Lindsay, PhilipC. Jack, Jr.,and Marcus A.Christian. Journal of Applied Psychology</ref> One possibility is that expertise in perceiving faces of particular races is associated with increased ability to extract information about the spatial relationships between different features<ref>Diamond &Carey, 1986; Rhodeset al.,1989</ref>. Further research using perceptual tasks could shed light on the specific cognitive processes involved in the other-race effect. <ref name="otherrace">..</ref>


=== Explanations ===
=== Explanations ===
{{main|Race and intelligence (Explanations)}}
[[Image:Blackwhite trendbirthyear.jpg|thumb|250px|Min-Hsiung Huang and Robert M. Hauser found that, controlling for social background, the Black-White test score gap narrowed significantly over the period from 1974 to 1998. For Whites, however, improvement in social background across time does not raise test scores correspondingly.<ref>''[http://www.ssc.wisc.edu/~hauser/lynn0810.pdf Convergent Trends in Black-White Test-Score Differentials in the U.S.: A Correction of Richard Lynn]'' Min-Hsiung Huang and Robert M. Hauser 2000</ref>]]
[[Image:TBC-BW-IQ-SES-withDiff.png|right|thumb|250px|This chart based on data analysis by Herrnstein and Murray, authors of ''[[The Bell Curve]]'', was used to present the idea that the test score gap between blacks and whites had not changed when adjusted for economic status. (1994) p. 288<ref>Reviewed in {{AYref|Neisser et al.|1996}}. Data from the [[National Longitudinal Surveys|NLSY]] as reported in figure adapted from {{AYref|Herrnstein and Murray|1994}}, p. 288.</ref>]]
[[Image:Heritability plants.jpeg|thumb|left|265px|The height of this "ordinary genetically varied corn" is 100% heritable, but the difference between the groups is totally environmental.<ref>[http://www.nyu.edu/gsas/dept/philo/faculty/block/papers/Heritability.html How Heritability Misleads about Race]</ref>]]


There is substantial debate about the influence of various environmental factors on [[Intelligence quotient|IQ]] test score differences between [[race]]s and ethnic groups in a given [[country]], and whether or not genetics may also play a role.


The consensus among intelligence researchers is that IQ differences between individuals of the same race reflects functionally and socially significant differences in the intelligence.<ref>{{Citation
|title=Gene variant may depress IQ of males
|url=http://www.azstarnet.com/news/105238
}}</ref><ref>{{Citation
|title=Link between gene and performance IQ
|url=http://www.whatsnextnetwork.com/technology/index.php/2007/02/27/link_between_and_performance_iq
}}</ref><ref>[http://www.dallasnews.com/sharedcontent/dws/news/healthscience/stories/DN-geneIQ_02nat.ART.State.Edition3.18e05d9a.html Gene may affect IQ in males, scientists say] Dallas Daily News</ref><ref>[http://www.newscientist.com/article/dn11831-parents-pass-on-genes-for-reasoning-and-memory.html Parents pass on genes for reasoning and memory] NewScientist.com</ref><ref>[http://www.gnxp.com/blog/2006/02/world-of-difference-richard-lynn-maps.php A World of Difference: Richard Lynn Maps World Intelligence] Gene Expression</ref><ref>[http://www.gnxp.com/blog/2005/09/this-is-bruce-lahns-brain-on-aspm-and.php Microcephalin, a Gene Regulating Brain Size, Continues to Evolve Adaptively in Humans] Gene Expressions</ref> There is still substantial debate about the influence of various environmental factors on [[Intelligence quotient|IQ]] test score differences between [[race]]s and ethnic groups in a given [[country]], and whether or not genetics may also play a role.
====Test bias====
====Test bias====
While the existence of average IQ test score differences has been a matter of accepted fact for decades a great deal of controversy exists among scholars over the question of whether these score differences reflected real differences in cognitive ability. Some claim that there is no evidence for test bias since IQ tests are equally good predictors of IQ-related factors (such as school performance) for U.S. Blacks and Whites.<ref name="APA">{{AYref|Neisser et al.|1996}}</ref> The performance differences persist in tests and testing situations in which care has been taken to eliminate bias.<ref name="APA">{{AYref|Neisser et al.|1996}}</ref> It has also been suggested that IQ tests are formulated in such a way as to disadvantage minorities.<ref name="APA">{{AYref|Neisser et al.|1996}}</ref> Controlled studies have shown that test construction does not substantially contribute to the IQ gap.<ref name="APA">{{AYref|Neisser et al.|1996}}</ref> Still, a 2007 study at [[Case Western Reserve University]] found that cultural differences in the provision of information account for racial differences in IQ. The study also found that test problems, similar to some problems found on conventional IQ tests, were only solvable on the basis of specific previous knowledge. Such specific knowledge based questions showed evidence of test bias since the performance on non-specific knowledge based questions did not always correlate with the performance on the knowledge based question.<ref>''Racial equality in intelligence: Predictions from a theory of intelligence as processing'' Joseph F. Fagan and Cynthia R. Holland. Intelligence Volume 35, Issue 4, July-August 2007, Pages 319-334</ref>
While the existence of average IQ test score differences has been a matter of accepted fact for decades a great deal of controversy exists among scholars over the question of whether these score differences reflected real differences in cognitive ability. Some claim that there is no evidence for test bias since IQ tests are equally good predictors of IQ-related factors (such as school performance) for U.S. Blacks and Whites.<ref name="APA">{{AYref|Neisser et al.|1996}}</ref> The performance differences persist in tests and testing situations in which care has been taken to eliminate bias.<ref name="APA">{{AYref|Neisser et al.|1996}}</ref> It has also been suggested that IQ tests are formulated in such a way as to disadvantage minorities.<ref name="APA">{{AYref|Neisser et al.|1996}}</ref> Controlled studies have shown that test construction does not substantially contribute to the IQ gap.<ref name="APA">{{AYref|Neisser et al.|1996}}</ref> Still, a 2007 study at [[Case Western Reserve University]] found that cultural differences in the provision of information account for racial differences in IQ. The study also found that test problems, similar to some problems found on conventional IQ tests, were only solvable on the basis of specific previous knowledge. Such specific knowledge based questions showed evidence of test bias since the performance on non-specific knowledge based questions did not always correlate with the performance on the knowledge based question.<ref>''Racial equality in intelligence: Predictions from a theory of intelligence as processing'' Joseph F. Fagan and Cynthia R. Holland. Intelligence Volume 35, Issue 4, July-August 2007, Pages 319-334</ref>

On a test (Black Intelligence Test of Cultural Homogeneity) oriented toward the language, attitudes, and life-styles of Afro-Americans, white students perform more poorly than blacks do on tests oriented toward white middle-class values, indicating that there are important dissimilarities in the cultural backgrounds of blacks and whites.<ref>''Racial Differences on a Black Intelligence Test'' Journal of Negro Education, 43, 4, 429-436, F 74</ref> Some argue that these findings indicate that test bias plays a role in producing the gaps in intelligence test scores.<ref>''IQ Tests and the Black Culture'' McNiel, Nathaniel D.</ref> [http://wilderdom.com/personality/intelligenceChitlingTestShort.html The Chitling Intelligence Test] is another example of a culturally biased test that tends to favor African Americans, although it should be noted that this test, despite its name, is more concerned with [[knowledge]] than [[intelligence]].<ref> Dove, A. The "Chitling" Test. From Lewis R. Aiken, Jr. (1971). Psychological and educational testings. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.</ref> These criticisms may not apply to "culture free" tests of intelligence. However, due to their cultural backgrounds some test takers do not have the familiarity with the language and culture of the psychological and educational tests that is implicitly assumed in the assessment procedure, even on "culture free" tests.<ref>''Special Issue on Advances in Testing Methodology from an International Perspective'' Applied Psychology. 53(2):215-236, April 2004.</ref> Beverly Daniel Tatum writes that dominant cultures often set the parameters by which minority cultures will be judged. Minority groups are labeled as substandard in significant ways, for example blacks have historically been characterized as less intelligent than whites. Tatum suggests that the ability to set these parameters is a form of [[White privilege (sociology)|white privilege]].<ref name="Tatum">{{cite book| first=Beverly Daniel |last=Tatum |authorlink=Beverly Daniel Tatum |year=1997 |title=Why are all the black kids sitting together in the cafeteria? And other conversations about race |location=New York |publisher=BasicBooks |isbn=9780465091270}}</ref>

====Environmental explanations====
Regarding the IQ gaps in the U.S., numerous explanations beside genetics have been proposed. Joel Wiesen lists more than a hundred.<ref>Joel Wiesen, "[http://appliedpersonnelresearch.com/papers/adimpact.pdf An Annotated List of Many Possible Reasons for the Black-White Mean Score Differences Seen With Many Cognitive Ability Tests: Notes to File]," Applied Personnel Research, March 18, 2005.</ref> It has been suggested by [[John Ogbu]] and others that [[African-American]] culture disfavors academic achievement and fosters an environment that is damaging to IQ.<ref>{{AYref|Boykin|1994}}</ref> Likewise, it is argued that the persistence of negative racial [[stereotypes]] reinforces this effect. Ogbu writes that the condition of being a "caste-like minority" affects motivation and achievement, depressing IQ.<ref>Ogbu JU, Davis A (2003). Black American Students in an Affluent Suburb: A Study of Academic Disengagement. Lawrence Erlbaum Publishers. ISBN 0-8058-4515-1</ref><ref name="Ogbu1978">Ogbu</ref> Although cultural differences may play a role in creating the gaps, much of the present gap found in IQ tests scores is likely the result of a combination of [[socioeconomic]] factors and health factors, such a [[breastfeeding]]. A 2006 study found that strongest and most robust predictors of intelligence were family income, parental education and breast feeding, with these three variables explaining 7.5% of the variation in intelligence at age 14.<ref>''[http://www.blackwell-synergy.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1365-3016.2006.00704.x Early life predictors of childhood intelligence: findings from the Mater-University study of pregnancy and its outcomes]'' Debbie A. Lawlor, Jake M. Najman, G. David Batty, Michael J. O'Callaghan, Gail M. Williams, William Bor (2006) Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology</ref> The impact of racial stereotypes has also been shown to play a key role. Making race salient in testing stations depresses the performance of minority students who belong to racial groups that have been historically stereotyped as less intelligent. (See:[[Race and intelligence (media portrayal)]]) Recent developments in intervention methods to counteract the impact of negative stereotypes have proved promising.

Arguing that IQ tests are often wrongly described as measuring "innate" rather than developed ability, {{AYref|Jencks and Phillips|1998}} write that this "labeling bias" causes people to inappropriately attribute the Black-White gap to "innate" differences.<ref>[http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/pages/frontline/shows/sats/interviews/jencks.html PBS Jencks Interview] "If we change the names of the tests, they still measure the same thing but it wouldn't convey this idea that somehow you've gotten the potential of somebody when you measured their IQ. And I think that creates a big bias, because the people who do badly on the tests are labeled as people with low potential in many people's minds and they sometimes even believe that about themselves."</ref>
They argue that non-cultural environmental factors cause gaps measured by the tests, rather than innate difference based on genetics, and that to use these tests as a measure of innate difference is misleading and improper.<ref>{{AYref|Jencks and Phillips|1998}} "... we find it hard to see how anyone reading these studies with an open mind could conclude that innate ability played a large role in the black-white gap."</ref>
They argue that non-cultural environmental factors cause gaps measured by the tests, rather than innate difference based on genetics, and that to use these tests as a measure of innate difference is misleading and improper.<ref>{{AYref|Jencks and Phillips|1998}} "... we find it hard to see how anyone reading these studies with an open mind could conclude that innate ability played a large role in the black-white gap."</ref>


=====Increases in IQ scores over time=====
====Increases in IQ scores over time====
[[Image:Blacktest score rise.jpg|250px|right|thumb|William T. Dickens and James R. Flynn write that blacks have gained 5 or 6 IQ points on non-Hispanic whites between 1972 and 2002. This graph shows the gains for various tests.<ref>''[http://www.brookings.edu/views/papers/dickens/20060619_IQ.pdf Black Americans reduce the racial IQ gap: Evidence from standardization samples]'' William T. Dickens and James R. Flynn. Oct. 2006</ref>]]
[[Image:Blacktest score rise.jpg|250px||thumb|William T. Dickens and James R. Flynn write that blacks have gained 5 or 6 IQ points on non-Hispanic whites between 1972 and 2002. This graph shows the gains for various tests.<ref>''[http://www.brookings.edu/views/papers/dickens/20060619_IQ.pdf Black Americans reduce the racial IQ gap: Evidence from standardization samples]'' William T. Dickens and James R. Flynn. Oct. 2006</ref>]]


The secular, international increase in test scores, commonly called the [[Flynn effect]], is seen by Flynn and others as reason to expect the eventual convergence of average black and white IQ scores. Flynn argues that the average IQ scores in several countries have increased about 3 points per decade during the 20th century, which he and others attribute predominantly to environmental causes.<ref>{{AYref|Flynn|1987}}, {{AYref|Flynn|1987b}}, {{AYref|Flynn|1999}}, {{AYref|Flynn|1999b}}</ref> This means, given the same test, the mean black American performance today could be higher than the mean white American performance in 1920, though the gains causing this appear to have occurred predominantly in the lower half of the IQ distribution.<ref>{{AYref|Colom et al.|2005}}</ref> If changes in environment can cause changes in IQ over time, they argue, then contemporary differences between groups could also be due to an unknown environmental factor. On the supposition that the effect started earlier for whites, because their social and economical conditions began to improve earlier than did those of blacks, they anticipate that the IQ gap among races might change in the future or is even now changing. An added complication to this hypothesis is the question of whether the secular IQ gains can be predominantly a real change in cognitive ability. Flynn's face-value answer to this question is "No",<ref>{{AYref|Flynn|1999}}</ref> and other researchers have found reason to concur. {{AYref|Wicherts et al.|2004}} wrote that "the gains cannot be explained solely by increases at the level of the latent variables (common factors), which IQ tests purport to measure".
{{Main|Flynn effect}}
The secular, international increase in test scores, commonly called the [[Flynn effect]], is seen by Flynn and others as reason to expect the eventual convergence of average black and white IQ scores. Flynn argues that the average IQ scores in several countries have increased about 3 points per decade during the 20th century, which he and others attribute predominantly to environmental causes.<ref>{{AYref|Flynn|1987}}, {{AYref|Flynn|1987b}}, {{AYref|Flynn|1999}}, {{AYref|Flynn|1999b}}</ref> This means, given the same test, the mean black American performance today could be higher than the mean white American performance in 1920, though the gains causing this appear to have occurred predominantly in the lower half of the IQ distribution.<ref>{{AYref|Colom et al.|2005}}</ref> If changes in environment can cause changes in IQ over time, they argue, then contemporary differences between groups could also be due to an unknown environmental factor. On the supposition that the effect started earlier for whites, because their social and economical conditions began to improve earlier than did those of blacks, they anticipate that the IQ gap among races might change in the future or is even now changing. An added complication to this hypothesis is the question of whether the secular IQ gains can be predominantly a real change in cognitive ability. Flynn's face-value answer to this question is "No",<ref>{{AYref|Flynn|1999}}</ref> and other researchers have found reason to concur. {{AYref|Wicherts et al.|2004}} wrote that "the gains cannot be explained solely by increases at the level of the latent variables (common factors), which IQ tests purport to measure". An analysis by {{AYref|Rushton|1999}} reported that the IQ increases associated with the Flynn effect did not produce changes in ''g'', which Rushton compares to the finding by {{AYref|Jensen|1998a}} that IQ increases associated with adoption likewise do not increase ''g''. {{AYref|Flynn|1999b}} disagrees with Rushton's analysis.


====Racism and discrimination====
{{AYref|Dickens and Flynn|2001}} have proposed a solution which rests on [[genotype-environment correlation|gene-environment correlation]], hypothesizing that small initial differences in environment cause feedback effects which magnify into large IQ differences. {{AYref|Rowe and Rodgers|2002}} and others find this hypothesis unsupported by the available evidence. {{AYref|Dickens and Flynn|2002}} respond to these criticisms. Such differences would need to develop before age 3, when the black-white IQ gap can be first detected.<ref>{{AYref|Rushton and Jensen|2005a}}</ref>
Researchers such as Jack Demaine find racial categorizations problematic in educational settings.<ref>''Race, Categorisation and Educational Achievement'' British Journal of Sociology of Education, Vol. 10, No. 2 (1989), pp. 195-214</ref> Racial categorizations, Jack Demaine writes, may have adverse impacts on the education of minorities. Similarly, Alastair Bonnett, Bruce Carrington state:

The [[Flynn effect]] consists of large documented worldwide increases in IQ scores for at least several decades. Attempted explanations have included improved nutrition, a trend towards smaller families, better education, greater environmental complexity, and [[heterosis]].

Comparing the Flynn effect (IQ differences within races over time) to contemporary IQ differences between races is contested; for example, one report concludes "the nature of the Flynn effect is qualitatively different from the nature of black-white differences in the United States," and that "the implications of the Flynn effect for black-white differences appear small" However, this refers to "measurement invariance", is not a statement about the role of genetics in the B-W gap, and is a relatively minor statement that not mentioned in the abstract.({{AYref|Wicherts et al.|2004}}).

A recent theory hypothesizes that fluid cognition (gF') may be separable from general intelligence, and that gF' may be very susceptible to environmental factors, in particular early childhood stress. Some IQ tests, especially those used with children, are poor measures of gF', which means that the effect of the environment on intelligence regarding racial differences, the Flynn effect, early childhood intervention, and life outcomes may have been underestimated in many studies. The article has received numerous peer commentaries for and against.<ref>[http://scholar.google.se/scholar?as_q=&num=10&btnG=S%C3%B6k+Scholar&as_epq=How+similar+are+fluid+cognition+and+general+intelligence&as_oq=&as_eq=&as_occt=any&as_sauthors=&as_publication=Behavioral+and+Brain+Sciences&as_ylo=&as_yhi=&as_allsubj=all&hl=sv&lr=]</ref>

A recent, newly available, large, and nationally representative data set find only very small (0.06 SD between whites and blacks) racial differences on measures for mental function for children aged eight to twelve months. These differences disappear when controlling for a limited set of factors such as differences in SES. "These findings pose a substantial challenge to the simplest, most direct, and most often articulated genetic stories regarding racial differences in mental function." "To the extent that there are any genetically-driven racial differences in intelligence, these gaps must either emerge after the age of one, or operate along dimensions not captured by this early test of mental cognition."<ref>[http://post.economics.harvard.edu/faculty/fryer/papers/fryer_levitt_ecls_babies.pdf]</ref>In their 2006 study, ''Black Americans reduce the racial IQ gap: Evidence from standardization samples'', William T. Dickens and James R. Flynn write that blacks have gained 5 or 6 IQ points on non-Hispanic whites between 1972 and 2002. Gains have been fairly uniform across the entire range of black cognitive ability.<ref>''[http://www.brookings.edu/views/papers/dickens/20060619_IQ.pdf Black Americans reduce the racial IQ gap: Evidence from standardization samples]'' William T. Dickens and James R. Flynn. Oct. 2006</ref>

=====Racism and discrimination=====
Reserchers such, as Jack Demaine find racial categorizations problematic in educational settings.<ref>''Race, Categorisation and Educational Achievement'' British Journal of Sociology of Education, Vol. 10, No. 2 (1989), pp. 195-214</ref> Racial categorizations, Jack Demaine writes, may have adverse impacts on the education of minorities. Similarly, Alastair Bonnett, Bruce Carrington state:


<blockquote>The collection of ethnic and racial statistics has become common in a growing number of institutional settings. Yet contemporary approaches to race and ethnicity suggest that the very process of compelling people to assign themselves to one of a small number of racial or ethnic 'boxes' is, at best, essentialist and, at worst, racist.<ref>''Fitting into Categories or Falling Between Them? Rethinking ethnic classification'' Alastair Bonnett, Bruce Carrington. British Journal of Sociology of Education Volume 21, Number 4 / December 1, 2000 Pages:487 - 500</ref></blockquote>
<blockquote>The collection of ethnic and racial statistics has become common in a growing number of institutional settings. Yet contemporary approaches to race and ethnicity suggest that the very process of compelling people to assign themselves to one of a small number of racial or ethnic 'boxes' is, at best, essentialist and, at worst, racist.<ref>''Fitting into Categories or Falling Between Them? Rethinking ethnic classification'' Alastair Bonnett, Bruce Carrington. British Journal of Sociology of Education Volume 21, Number 4 / December 1, 2000 Pages:487 - 500</ref></blockquote>


======Stereotype threat======
=====Stereotype threat=====
{{main|Stereotype threat}}
{{main|Stereotype threat}}
[[Image:Stereotype threat bw.jpg|thumb|250px|right|An experiment on college students in 1995 showed the impact of [[Stereotype threat]] by asking students to fill out a form before taking the test indicating their race. The scores in this graph have been adjusted by SAT.<ref>''The Effects of Stereotype Threat on the Standardized Test Performance of College Students'' J Aronson, [[Claude Steele|CM Steele]], MF Salinas, MJ Lustina - Readings About the Social Animal, 8th edition, E. Aronson </ref>]]
[[Image:Stereotype threat bw.jpg|thumb|250px|right|An experiment on college students in 1995 showed the impact of [[Stereotype threat]] by asking students to fill out a form before taking the test indicating their race. The scores in this graph have been adjusted by SAT.<ref>''The Effects of Stereotype Threat on the Standardized Test Performance of College Students'' J Aronson, [[Claude Steele|CM Steele]], MF Salinas, MJ Lustina - Readings About the Social Animal, 8th edition, E. Aronson </ref>]]
<!-- Deleted image removed: [[Image:Claude Steele.jpg|right|140px|thumb|'''Claude Mason Steele''' is a social psychologist at [[Stanford University]] known for his work on [[stereotype threat]]]] -->


Stereotype threat is the fear that one's behavior will confirm an existing [[stereotype]] of a group with which one identifies. This fear may in turn lead to an impairment of performance (Aronson, Wilson, & Akert, 2005). Stereotype threat has been documented by the social psychologists Claude Steele, Joshua Aronson, Irwin Katz, and Steven Spencer, who have conducted several studies on this topic.
Stereotype threat is the fear that one's behavior will confirm an existing [[stereotype]] of a group with which one identifies. This fear may in turn lead to an impairment of performance (Aronson, Wilson, & Akert, 2005). Stereotype threat has been documented by the social psychologists Claude Steele, Joshua Aronson, Irwin Katz, and Steven Spencer, who have conducted several studies on this topic.
Line 188: Line 194:
In a paper prepared for an [[American Psychological Association|APA]] convention, Steele writes: "''Thus the predicament of 'stereotype vulnerability': The group members then know that anything about them or anything they do that fits the stereotype can be taken as confirming it as self-characteristic, in the eyes of others, and perhaps even in their own eyes. This vulnerability amounts to a jeopardy of double devaluation: once for whatever bad thing the stereotype-fitting behavior or feature would say about anyone, and again for its confirmation of the bad things alleged in the stereotype.''"
In a paper prepared for an [[American Psychological Association|APA]] convention, Steele writes: "''Thus the predicament of 'stereotype vulnerability': The group members then know that anything about them or anything they do that fits the stereotype can be taken as confirming it as self-characteristic, in the eyes of others, and perhaps even in their own eyes. This vulnerability amounts to a jeopardy of double devaluation: once for whatever bad thing the stereotype-fitting behavior or feature would say about anyone, and again for its confirmation of the bad things alleged in the stereotype.''"


Steele and Aronson are not first to test stereotype threat. During the 1960’s Irwin Katz, psychologist, suggested that stereotype threat could also influence performance on IQ tests. Katz found that Blacks were able to score better an IQ subtest if the test was presented as a test of eye-hand coordination. Blacks also scored higher on an IQ test when they believe the test will be compared to that of other blacks.<ref>''Review of Evidence Relating to Effects of Desegregation on the Intellectual Performance of Negroes'' I Katz - American Psychologist, 1964</ref> Katz concluded that his subjects were thoroughly aware of the judgment of intellectual inferiority held by many white Americans. With little expectation of overruling this judgment, their motivation was low, and so were their scores.<ref>[http://www.time.com/time/magazine/article/0,9171,902708,00.html Race and IQ] TIME. Monday, Sep. 07, 1970</ref> Paul Sackett, a psychologist agrees that stereotype threat is a real phenomenon and that it is is a potentially important contributor to the racial achievement gap. He cautions however, that these findings may be widely misinterpreted to mean that eliminating stereotype threat eliminates the entire Black-White performance gap, and encourages researchers to continue their study of this and other phenomena. <ref>Sackett, P. R., Hardison, C. M. and Cullen, M. J. (Apr 2005). "On Interpreting Research on Stereotype Threat and Test Performance". American Psychologist 60 (3): 271-272. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.60.3.271 DOI:10.1037/0003-066x.60.3.271].</ref>
Steele and Aronson are not first to test stereotype threat. During the Irwin Katz, psychologist, suggested that stereotype threat could also influence performance on IQ tests. Katz found that Blacks were able to score better an IQ subtest if the test was presented as a test of eye-hand coordination. Blacks also scored higher on an IQ test when they believe the test will be compared to that of other blacks.<ref>''Review of Evidence Relating to Effects of Desegregation on the Intellectual Performance of Negroes'' I Katz - American Psychologist, 1964</ref> Katz concluded that his subjects were thoroughly aware of the judgment of intellectual inferiority held by many white Americans. With little expectation of overruling this judgment, their motivation was low, and so were their scores.<ref>[http://www.time.com/time/magazine/article/0,9171,902708,00.html Race and IQ] TIME. Monday, Sep. 07, 1970</ref> Paul Sackett, a psychologist agrees that stereotype threat is a real phenomenon and that it is is a potentially important contributor to the racial achievement gap. He cautions however, that these findings may be widely misinterpreted to mean that eliminating stereotype threat eliminates the entire Black-White performance gap, and encourages researchers to continue their study of this and other phenomena. <ref>Sackett, P. R., Hardison, C. M. and Cullen, M. J. (Apr 2005). "On Interpreting Research on Stereotype Threat and Test Performance". American Psychologist 60 (3): 271-272. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.60.3.271 DOI:10.1037/0003-066x.60.3.271].</ref>


=====Physiological responses to racism=====
Since stereotype threat appears to be one key contributing factors to the gaps in test scores, researchers Geoffrey L. Cohen, Julio Garcia, Nancy Apfel, and Allison Master proposed intervention methods to address the problem in 2006. The intervention, a brief in-class writing assignment, significantly improved the grades of African American students and reduced the racial achievement gap by 40%. These results suggest that the racial achievement gap, a major social concern in the United States, could be ameliorated by the use of timely and targeted social-psychological interventions.<ref>''[http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/abstract/sci;313/5791/1307 Reducing the Racial Achievement Gap: A Social-Psychological Intervention]'' Science 1 September 2006: Vol. 313. no. 5791, pp. 1307 - 1310</ref>
{{seealso|Race and health}}
Stereotype threat can result in physiological responses that can be measured objectively. For example, a study by Blascovich J, Spencer SJ, Quinn D and Steele C. reported that African Americans under stereotype threat exhibited larger increases in arterial blood pressure during an academic test, and performed more poorly on difficult test items. Some researchers feel this may explain the higher death rates from [[hypertension]] related disorders among African Americans.<ref>African Americans and high blood pressure: the role of stereotype threat. Blascovich J, Spencer SJ, Quinn D and Steele C. Department of Psychology, [[University of California]], Santa Barbara 93106, USA.</ref> A study by Toni Schmader and Michael Johns reported that stereotype threat can effectively reduce working memory capacity, another factor in poor test performance.<ref>''Converging Evidence That Stereotype Threat Reduces Working Memory Capacity'' Toni Schmader and Michael Johns 2003</ref> Stereotype threat may undermine intellectual performance by triggering a disruptive mental load. Jean-Claude Croizet, Gérard Després, Marie-Eve Gauzins, Pascal Huguet, Jacques-Philippe Leyens and Alain Méot reported increased heart rates for test subjects operating under stereotype threat.<ref>''Stereotype Threat Undermines Intellectual Performance by Triggering a Disruptive Mental Load'' 2004 Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.</ref>


=====Quality of education=====
Some researches have written that studies that find test performance gaps between races even after adjusting for education level, such as the analysis found in ''The Bell Curve'', fail to adjust for the quality of education. Not all high school graduates or college graduates have received the same quality of education. A 2006 study reported that that years of education is an inadequate measure of the educational experience among multicultural elders, and that adjusting for quality of education greatly reduced the overall effect of racial differences on the tests.<ref>''[http://www.journals.cambridge.org/action/displayAbstract?fromPage=online&aid=106211 Reading level attenuates differences in neuropsychological test performance between African American and White elders]'' JENNIFER J. MANLY, DIANE M. JACOBS, [[Pegah Touradji|PEGAH TOURADJI]], SCOTT A. SMALL and YAAKOV STERN</ref> A 2004 study reported that quality of education and cultural experience influence how older African Americans approach neuropsychological tasks and concluded that adjustment for these variables may improve specificity of neuropsychological measures.<ref>''Acculturation, Reading Level, and Neuropsychological Test Performance Among African American Elders
'' Jennifer J. Manly‌, Desiree A. Byrd‌, Pegah Touradji‌, Yaakov Stern‌</ref> Yet another study reported that, although significant differences were observed between the ethnic groups when matched for years of education, equating for literacy level eliminated all performance differences between African Americans and Whites on both cancellation tasks which assess visual scanning.<ref>''Cancellation test performance in African American, Hispanic, and White elderly'' DESIREE A. BYRD, PEGAH TOURADJI, MING-XIN TANG and JENNIFER J. MANLY</ref>(Like reaction time tests cancellation task tests are sometimes regarded as "culture free" tests of intelligence.) Eric A. Hanushek and Steven G. Rivkin wrote in their 2006 book that unequal distributions of inexperienced teachers and of racial concentrations in schools can explain all of the increased achievement gap between grades 3 and 8.<ref>''School Quality and the Black-White Achievement Gap'' Eric A. Hanushek and Steven G. Rivkin 2006</ref>


A 2004 study in [[South Africa]] found highly significant effects for both level and quality of education within the black African first language groups taking the Wechsler IQ tests. Scores black African first language groups with advantaged education were comparable with the US standardization, whereas scores for black African first language participants with disadvantaged education were significantly lower than this. The study cautioned that faulty conclusions may be drawn about the effects of ethnicity and the potential for [[neuropsychological]] [[misdiagnosis]].<ref>''Cross-cultural Effects on IQ Test Performance: A Review and Preliminary Normative Indications on WAIS-III Test Performance
'' Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology Volume 26, Number 7 / October 2004</ref>


======Caste-like minorities======
===== =====
Roslyn Arlin Mickelson writes that racial discrimination in education arises from actions of institutions or individual state actors, their attitudes and ideologies, or processes that systematically treat students from different racial/ethnic groups disparately or inequitably.<ref>''When Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of Racial Discrimination? A Social Science Perspective'' by Roslyn Arlin Mickelson University of North Carolina at Charlotte</ref> Despite advancement in education reform efforts, to this day African American students continue to experience inequities within the educational system. Hala Elhoweris , Kagendo Mutua, Negmeldin Alsheikh and Pauline Holloway conducted a study of the effect of students' ethnicity on teachers' educational decision making. The results of this study indicated that the student's ethnicity did make a difference in the teachers' referral decisions for [[Gifted education|gifted and talented]] educational programs.<ref>''Effect of Children's Ethnicity on Teachers' Referral and Recommendation Decisions in Gifted and Talented Programs'' Journal article by Negmeldin Alsheikh, Hala Elhoweris, Pauline Holloway, Kagendo Mutua; Remedial and Special Education, Vol. 26, 2005</ref>Recently, a number of scholars have examined the issue of disproportionate representation of minority students in [[special education]] programs <ref>(Salend, Garrick Duhaney, & Montgomery, 2002; Townsend, 2002)</ref><ref>''Racial Inequity in Special Education.'' Losen, Daniel J., Ed.; Orfield, Gary, Ed. Harvard Education Publishing Group.</ref>
[[Image:John Ogbu.jpg|thumb|140px|right|[[John Ogbu|John Uzo Ogbu]]<br> Anthropologist known for his theories on "caste-like minorities" and "The effort gap"]]


Teachers' perceptions of a students cultural background may effect school achievement. African American students with African American cultural backgrounds, for example, have been found to benefit from culturally responsive teaching.<ref>(Gay, 2000; Irvine & Armento, 2001; Ladson-Billings, 1994, 2001)</ref> In a 2003 study researchers found that teachers perceived students with African American culture-related movement styles as lower in achievement, higher in aggression, and more likely to need special education services than students with standard movement styles irrespective of race or other academic indicators. <ref>''The Effects of African American Movement Styles on Teachers' Perceptions and Reactions'' Journal article by Scott T. Bridgest, Audrey Davis Mccray, La Vonne I. Neal, Gwendolyn Webb-Johnson; Journal of Special Education, Vol. 37, 2003</ref>
John Ogbu writes that [[Caste|caste-like]] minorities are not the same as other racial minorities. Caste-like minorities are incorporated into a country involuntarily and permanently. These include Blacks, [[American Indians]], [[Mexicans]], Native Hawaiians, Puerto Ricans and others. Membership is a low caste acquired at birth and retained permanently. Caste members are regarded by the white majority as inferior and not desirable as neighbors or workmates. Often, they lack political power and are economically subordinate. They face a job ceiling, and are not hired on the basis of training and skills like other minorities. Caste-like groups also reject the ideology and beliefs of the dominant group culture. They believe their problems are due to the "system" and racism than their own inadequacies. They may develop a "collective institutional discrimination perspective". This leads them into channel efforts into collective struggle.<ref>Ogbu, J. U., ''The Consequences of the American Caste System, In U. Neisser (Ed.), The School Achievement of Minority Children: New Perspectives'', 1986</ref>


Ellis Cose writes that low expectations may have a negative impact on the achievement of minorities. He writes that black people did not need to read ''The Bell Curve'' to be aware of the low expectations held for them by the majority culture. He recalls examples of low expectations from his teachers in school who regarded his use of [[AAVE]] as "laziness" and teachers who did not feel it was important to purchase new text books because they did not expect the students to be able to read anything complex. He contrasts these low expectations with the high expectations philosophy of [[Xavier University of Louisiana|Xavier University]] where, using the ideas Whimbey articulated in his book ''Intelligence can be Taught'' teachers created a program called SOAR. SOAR raised the performance of black students and lead Xavier to become the university that sends the greatest number of black students to medical school in the United States. The SOAR program produced gains equivalent to 120 points on an SAT test. Cose writes that "..we must treat people, whatever their color, as if they have unlimited intellectual capacity."<ref>''Color-Blind'' Ellis Cose. Page 50</ref>
Like Blacks and Hispanics in the U.S., minorities in non-US societies show achievement gaps (such as the [[Māori]] in New Zealand, [[Australian Aborigine|aboriginals]] in Australia, scheduled castes ("[[Dalit (outcaste)|untouchables]]") in India, non-European Jews in Israel, and the [[Burakumin]] in Japan). The most prominent finding cited is that Northern Irish Catholics used to score about 15 points lower than Protestants. Similarly, Irish, Italian and Polish immigrants in the U.S. are reported to have all scored about 80 in the beginning of the 19th century, but now tend to reach 100. The same is true of persons from rural versus urban areas in general (see e.g., [http://www.mugu.com/cgi-bin/Upstream/Issues/bell-curve/sowell.html this article] by conservative columnist and economist [[Thomas Sowell]] and [http://dienekes.angeltowns.net/articles/greekiq/ this page] on European and Greek IQ. More arguments of the kind are to be found [http://home.att.net/~Resurgence/L-inferiorIQ.htm here]).


====Caste====
This table illustrates how social status or [[caste]] position is related to test scores and school success in nations around the world. Source: ''[http://press.princeton.edu/chapters/s5877.html Inequality by Design: Cracking the Bell Curve Myth]'' by Claude S. Fischer, Michael Hout, Martín Sánchez Jankowski, Samuel R. Lucas, Ann Swidler, and Kim Vos<ref name="bell myth">''[http://press.princeton.edu/chapters/s5877.html Inequality by Design: Cracking the Bell Curve Myth]'' by Claude S. Fischer, Michael Hout, Martín Sánchez Jankowski, Samuel R. Lucas, Ann Swidler, and Kim Vos. Page 192. (The footnotes given are also from this book.)</ref>
This table illustrates how social status or [[caste]] position is related to test scores and school success in nations around the world. Source: ''[http://press.princeton.edu/chapters/s5877.html Inequality by Design: Cracking the Bell Curve Myth]'' by Claude S. Fischer, Michael Hout, Martín Sánchez Jankowski, Samuel R. Lucas, Ann Swidler, and Kim Vos<ref name="bell myth">''[http://press.princeton.edu/chapters/s5877.html Inequality by Design: Cracking the Bell Curve Myth]'' by Claude S. Fischer, Michael Hout, Martín Sánchez Jankowski, Samuel R. Lucas, Ann Swidler, and Kim Vos. Page 192. (The footnotes given are also from this book.)</ref>


Line 328: Line 342:
These results, just like the inferior test scores of Eastern and Southern Europeans immigrants in the United States 75 years ago, may represent a social division that leads to the gaps in test scores, rather than a pre-established and "natural" hierarchy of "races." In other words, these divisions, are closely aligned with local "social constructs" of race, the outcomes for ethnic groups are, in the opinion of these authors, a result of the social structure rather than confirmation of its validity.<ref name="bell myth">..</ref>
These results, just like the inferior test scores of Eastern and Southern Europeans immigrants in the United States 75 years ago, may represent a social division that leads to the gaps in test scores, rather than a pre-established and "natural" hierarchy of "races." In other words, these divisions, are closely aligned with local "social constructs" of race, the outcomes for ethnic groups are, in the opinion of these authors, a result of the social structure rather than confirmation of its validity.<ref name="bell myth">..</ref>


======Quality of education======
========
{{main|Health and intelligence}}
Some researches have written that studies that find test performance gaps between races even after adjusting for education level, such as the analysis found in ''The Bell Curve'', fail to adjust for the quality of education. Not all high school graduates or college graduates have received the same quality of education. A 2006 study reported that that years of education is an inadequate measure of the educational experience among multicultural elders, and that adjusting for quality of education greatly reduced the overall effect of racial differences on the tests.<ref>''[http://www.journals.cambridge.org/action/displayAbstract?fromPage=online&aid=106211 Reading level attenuates differences in neuropsychological test performance between African American and White elders]'' JENNIFER J. MANLY, DIANE M. JACOBS, [[Pegah Touradji|PEGAH TOURADJI]], SCOTT A. SMALL and YAAKOV STERN</ref> A 2004 study reported that quality of education and cultural experience influence how older African Americans approach neuropsychological tasks and concluded that adjustment for these variables may improve specificity of neuropsychological measures.<ref>''Acculturation, Reading Level, and Neuropsychological Test Performance Among African American Elders
{{main|Race and health}}
'' Jennifer J. Manly‌, Desiree A. Byrd‌, Pegah Touradji‌, Yaakov Stern‌</ref> Yet another study reported that, although significant differences were observed between the ethnic groups when matched for years of education, equating for literacy level eliminated all performance differences between African Americans and Whites on both cancellation tasks which assess visual scanning.<ref>''Cancellation test performance in African American, Hispanic, and White elderly'' DESIREE A. BYRD, PEGAH TOURADJI, MING-XIN TANG and JENNIFER J. MANLY</ref> (Like reaction time tests cancellation task tests are sometimes regarded as "culture free" tests of intelligence.) Eric A. Hanushek and Steven G. Rivkin wrote in their 2006 book that unequal distributions of inexperienced teachers and of racial concentrations in schools can explain all of the increased achievement gap between grades 3 and 8.<ref>''School Quality and the Black-White Achievement Gap'' Eric A. Hanushek and Steven G. Rivkin 2006</ref>
Regarding the IQ gaps in the U.S., numerous explanations beside genetics have been proposed. Joel Wiesen lists more than a hundred.<ref>Joel Wiesen, "[http://appliedpersonnelresearch.com/papers/adimpact.pdf An Annotated List of Many Possible Reasons for the Black-White Mean Score Differences Seen With Many Cognitive Ability Tests: Notes to File]," Applied Personnel Research, March 18, 2005.</ref> Increased rates of low birth weight babies and lower rates of breastfeeding in Blacks as compared to Whites are some factors of many that have been proposed to affect the IQ gap. The [[Flynn effect]] is often cited as evidence that average IQ scores have changed greatly and rapidly, for reasons poorly understood, noting that average IQ in the US may have been below 75 before the start of this effect, and thus some argue that the IQ gap between races could change in the future or is changing, especially if the effect started earlier for Whites.


====Genetics====
A 2004 study in [[South Africa]] found highly significant effects for both level and quality of education within the black African first language groups taking the Wechsler IQ tests. Scores black African first language groups with advantaged education were comparable with the US standardization, whereas scores for black African first language participants with disadvantaged education were significantly lower than this. The study cautioned that faulty conclusions may be drawn about the effects of ethnicity and the potential for [[neuropsychological]] [[misdiagnosis]].<ref>''Cross-cultural Effects on IQ Test Performance: A Review and Preliminary Normative Indications on WAIS-III Test Performance
: ''See also [[Race and genetics#The genome and intelligence|The genome and intelligence]] ''
'' Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology Volume 26, Number 7 / October 2004</ref>
A few of the notable proponents of the partly genetic hypothesis are [[Raymond B. Cattell]], [[Arthur Jensen]] and [[Hans Eysenck]].


[[J. Philippe Rushton|Rushton]] and [[Arthur Jensen|Jensen]] examined 10 categories of research evidence from around the world to contrast "a hereditarian model" (50% genetic-50% cultural) and a culture-only model (0% genetic-100% cultural). In the article "''Thirty Years of Research on Race Differences in Cognitive Ability''" published in the [[APA]] journal ''Psychology, Public Policy and Law'' they cite the following evidence to support the hereditarian model:<ref>http://psychology.uwo.ca/faculty/rushtonpdfs/PPPL1.pdf Thirty Years of Research on Race Differences in Cognitive Ability</ref><ref>http://www.innovations-report.de/html/berichte/studien/bericht-43536.html Black-White-East Asian IQ differences at least 50% genetic, major law review journal concludes</ref>
======Racial discrimination in education======
Roslyn Arlin Mickelson writes that racial discrimination in education arises from actions of institutions or individual state actors, their attitudes and ideologies, or processes that systematically treat students from different racial/ethnic groups disparately or inequitably.<ref>''When Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of Racial Discrimination? A Social Science Perspective'' by Roslyn Arlin Mickelson University of North Carolina at Charlotte</ref> Despite advancement in education reform efforts, to this day African American students continue to experience inequities within the educational system. Hala Elhoweris , Kagendo Mutua , Negmeldin Alsheikh and Pauline Holloway conducted a study of the effect of students' ethnicity on teachers' educational decision making. The results of this study indicated that the student's ethnicity did make a difference in the teachers' referral decisions for [[Gifted education|gifted and talented]] educational programs.<ref>''Effect of Children's Ethnicity on Teachers' Referral and Recommendation Decisions in Gifted and Talented Programs'' Journal article by Negmeldin Alsheikh, Hala Elhoweris, Pauline Holloway, Kagendo Mutua; Remedial and Special Education, Vol. 26, 2005</ref>Recently, a number of scholars have examined the issue of disproportionate representation of minority students in [[special education]] programs <ref>(Salend, Garrick Duhaney, & Montgomery, 2002; Townsend, 2002)</ref><ref>''Racial Inequity in Special Education.'' Losen, Daniel J., Ed.; Orfield, Gary, Ed. Harvard Education Publishing Group.</ref>


Some scholars have proposed that in order to make a racial hypothesis about intelligence the genes for intelligence need to be identified along with their frequencies in the various populations.
Teachers' perceptions of a students cultural background may effect school achievement. African American students with African American cultural backgrounds, for example, have been found to benefit from culturally responsive teaching.<ref>(Gay, 2000; Irvine & Armento, 2001; Ladson-Billings, 1994, 2001)</ref> In a 2003 study researchers found that teachers perceived students with African American culture-related movement styles as lower in achievement, higher in aggression, and more likely to need special education services than students with standard movement styles irrespective of race or other academic indicators. <ref>''The Effects of African American Movement Styles on Teachers' Perceptions and Reactions'' Journal article by Scott T. Bridgest, Audrey Davis Mccray, La Vonne I. Neal, Gwendolyn Webb-Johnson; Journal of Special Education, Vol. 37, 2003</ref>
However recent studies attempting to find regions in the [[human genome|genome]] relating to intelligence have had little success. A recent study used several hundred people in two groups, one with a very high IQ, average 160, and a control group with an average IQ of 102. By the fifth step the study could not find a single gene that was related to intelligence. Critics of these studies say the failure to find a specific gene associated with intelligence is indicative of the complex nature of intelligence. They contend that intelligence is probably under the influence of several genes. Some estimate that as much as 40% of the genome may contribute to intelligence.<ref>[http://www.meb.ki.se/education/epi/descriptions/gen_mol_epi/cognitive_paper.pdf A Genome-Wide Scan of 1842 DNA Markers for Allelic Associations With General Cognitive Ability: A Five-Stage Design Using DNA Pooling and Extreme Selected Groups]</ref>


Recently scientists at the University of Chicago identified two genes, [[microcephalin]] and [[ASPM]]. Mutations in these genes are associated with brain size abnormality, [[microcephaly]]. The normal variants are found at high frequencies in Asian and European populations but they are not found among Sub-Saharan Africans. The scientists stated that microcephalin may have arisen some 37,000 years ago coinciding with upper paleolithic transitions in Europe. They also stated that a variant of APSM arose about 5,800 years ago roughly correlating with the development of written language, spread of agriculture and development of cities. They thus believe these two genes conferred some cognitive abilities upon Asians and Europeans. <ref>[http://www.nytimes.com/2005/09/09/science/09brain.html?ex=1182139200&en=2fe58cf014a688dc&ei=5070 Brain May Still Be Evolving, Studies Hint]</ref>
Ellis Cose writes that low expectations may have a negative impact on the achievement of minorities. He writes that black people did not need to read ''The Bell Curve'' to be aware of the low expectations held for them by the majority culture. He recalls examples of low expectations from his teachers in school who regarded his use of [[AAVE]] as "laziness" and teachers who did not feel it was important to purchase new text books because they did not expect the students to be able to read anything complex. He contrasts these low expectations with the high expectations philosophy of [[Xavier University of Louisiana|Xavier University]] where, using the ideas Whimbey articulated in his book ''Intelligence can be Taught'' teachers created a program called SOAR. SOAR raised the performance of black students and lead Xavier to become the university that sends the greatest number of black students to medical school in the United States. The SOAR program produced gains equivalent to 120 points on an SAT test. Cose writes that "..we must treat people, whatever their color, as if they have unlimited intellectual capacity."<ref>''Color-Blind'' Ellis Cose. Page 50</ref>


Other scholars have criticized the University of Chicago scientists because they made claims about these genes without undertaking any direct experimentation to test their hypothesis on increased intelligence and brain size. Subsequently when these experiments were done, no relationship was found between these genes and intelligence or brain size.
=====Socio-economic factors=====
<ref>[http://hmg.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/abstract/ddl487v1 The ongoing adaptive evolution of ASPM and Microcephalin is not explained by increased intelligence]</ref><ref>[http://hmg.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/full/15/12/2025 Normal variants of Microcephalin and ASPM do not account for brain size variability]</ref> Critics of these studies also say that as long as social and environmental disparities between the races exist it will be impossible to scientifically test whether there are any genetic differences in IQ between the various populations. They propose that if the historical effects of poverty and social bigotry were eliminated and differences in IQ between the races still persisted then there might be some utility in such research.<ref>[http://www.amazon.com/gp/reader/0525948252/ The race myth,] [[Joseph Graves]], page 183 ISBN 0452286581</ref>
[[Image:Blackwhite trendbirthyear.jpg|thumb|250px|Min-Hsiung Huang and Robert M. Hauser found that, controlling for social background, the Black-White test score gap narrowed significantly over the period from 1974 to 1998. For Whites, however, improvement in social background across time does not raise test scores correspondingly. <ref>''[http://www.ssc.wisc.edu/~hauser/lynn0810.pdf Convergent Trends in Black-White Test-Score Differentials in the U.S.: A Correction of Richard Lynn]'' Min-Hsiung Huang and Robert M. Hauser 2000</ref>]]


===Theories on the intelligence of Ashkenazi Jews===
[[Image:TBC-BW-IQ-SES-withDiff.png|right|thumb|250px|A graph from ''[[The Bell Curve]]'' and book that suggested that the black/white gap has not narrowed.]]
{{See also|Ashkenazi intelligence}}
A link between disease mutations specific to the [[Ashkenazi Jews]] and high IQ scores has been suggested by scientists at the University of Utah, who cite evidence that sphingolipid disorders promote the growth and interconnection of brain cells and that mutations in the DNA repair genes, involved in some Ashkenazic diseases, may also unleash growth of neurons. The researchers predict that these disease mutations will enhance IQ in heterozygotes. This prediction is based on evidence that selection pressure has increased the frequency of the disease mutations in the reproductively isolated Ashkenazi population in medieval times. The hypothesis has not yet been empirically tested.<ref>http://homepage.mac.com/harpend/.Public/AshkenaziIQ.jbiosocsci.pdf</ref>


Ashkenazi Jews have been reported to score 0.75 to 1.0 standard deviations above the general European average, corresponding to an IQ 112-115.<ref>[http://homepage.mac.com/harpend/.Public/AshkenaziIQ.jbiosocsci.pdf]</ref><ref>http://www.astarshop.com/j_dis.pdf</ref><ref>[http://www.economist.com/science/displaystory.cfm?story_id=E1_QDPNGPR]</ref>
IQ is correlated with economic factors. Blacks and Hispanics suffer poorer economic conditions than Whites. It has been suggested that the effects of poverty are responsible for some or all of the IQ gap. However, in the American Psychological Association report {{A(Y)ref|Neisser et al.|1996}} argue that economics cannot be the whole explanation. According to Richard J. Herrnstein and Charles Murray, authors of ''The Bell Curve'', to the moderate extent that IQ and income are related, it appears that IQ determines income, and not the other way around ({{AYref|Murray|1998}}). There are, however, many other potential Socio-economic factors factors beside income.


According to Richard Lynn the most accurate reading of the IQ of Jews in Britain is 110. Lynn proposes that the overrepresentation of Jews as Nobel Prize Winners and as Fellows of the Royal Society can be partly explained by the higher average Jewish IQ because comparatively small differences in average intelligence can become very large differences in the very high I.Q ranges. Notable Jewish intellectuals include [[Einstein]], [[Freud]] and [[Marx]]. Jews are over-represented among Nobel prize-winners by a factor of 8.0 in Britain and 12.3 in the United States.<ref>[http://www.sciencedirect.com/science?_ob=ArticleURL&_udi=B6W4M-4JYTRWC-2&_user=10&_coverDate=12%2F31%2F2006&_rdoc=1&_fmt=&_orig=search&_sort=d&view=c&_acct=C000050221&_version=1&_urlVersion=0&_userid=10&md5=b1d721140bda47fb8bca4a1200519d64 On the high intelligence and cognitive achievements of Jews in Britain]</ref><ref>Lynn, R. and Longley, D. (2006). "On the high intelligence and cognitive achievements of Jews in Britain." Intelligence, 34, 541-547.</ref>
Researchers have reported that many American Blacks and Hispanics are not given sufficient opportunity to learn language and thinking skills during the first three years of life, possibly due to economic status. The first three years are especially critical years for neural development of the brain, and previous studies have shown that when human children were deprived of most or all language skills at an early age, they never developed the ability to master language at a later age; if they only mastered a small amount of language and thinking skills at a young age, then they could only make small improvements in later years. A recent study has shown that many American Blacks and Hispanics are raised in homes where their parents speak relatively few sentences, and the sentences usually show only simple grammar. As a result, their children never hear millions of words during the time when their brains are developing linguistic skills. Without this linguistic input during their developing years, many are observed to quickly fall behind, and they can never catch up. Children in poorer welfare families, which includes a higher percentage of many minority populations, apparently hear up to 30 million fewer words by age three than children in higher income, usually White, families. (Source: ''The Early Catastrophe: The 30 Million Word Gap by Age 3'')


===Interpretations===
Work by {{A(Y)ref|Carneiro et al.|2005}} on average Black-Hispanic-White differences in IQ, education, and income casts doubt on conventional explanations of Black-White differences:
{{main|Race and intelligence (interpretations)}}
<blockquote>Hispanic children start with cognitive and noncognitive deficits similar to those of black children. They also grow up in similarly disadvantaged environments and are likely to attend schools of similar quality. Hispanics complete much less schooling than blacks. Nevertheless, the ability growth by years of schooling is much higher for Hispanics than for blacks. By the time they reach adulthood, Hispanics have significantly higher test scores than do blacks. Conditional on test scores, there is no evidence of an important Hispanic-white wage gap. Our analysis of the Hispanic data illuminates the traditional study of black-white differences and casts doubt on many conventional explanations of these differences because they do not apply to Hispanics, who also suffer from many of the same disadvantages. The failure of the Hispanic-white gap to widen with schooling or age casts doubt on the claim that poor schools and bad neighborhoods are the reasons for the slow growth rate of black test scores.</blockquote>
Given the observed differences in IQ scores between certain groups, a great deal of debate revolves around the significance of these observations. Various interpretations of test data lead to a multitude of conflicting conclusions as to which specific explanations the data support.


Some people have attributed differential economic growth between nations to differences in the intelligence of their populations. One example is Richard Lynn's ''[[IQ and the Wealth of Nations]]''. The book is sharply criticized in the peer-reviewed paper ''The Impact of National IQ on Income and Growth''.<ref> Thomas Volken, "[http://www.suz.unizh.ch/volken/ThomasVolken/pdfs/IQWealthNation.pdf The Impact of National IQ on Income and Growth]."</ref> Another peer-reviewed paper, ''Intelligence, Human Capital, and Economic Growth: An Extreme-Bounds Analysis'', finds a strong connection between intelligence and economic growth.<ref>{{AYref|Jones and Schneider|2005}}</ref> It has been argued that East Asian nations underachieve compared to IQ scores.
A recent 1996 study using multiple socio-economic factors have accounted for 80% of the gap, and suggest that any remaining gap is statistically insignificant.<ref>[http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28199604%2967%3A2%3C396%3AEDICIT%3E2.0.CO%3B2-6 Ethnic Differences in Children's Intelligence Test Scores: Role of Economic Deprivation, Home Environment, and Maternal Characteristics], Jeanne Brooks-Gunn; Pamela K. Klebanov; Greg J. Duncan Child Development, Vol. 67, No. 2. (Apr., 1996), pp. 396-408.]</ref>


[[Jared Diamond]]'s ''[[Guns, Germs and Steel]]'' instead argues that historical differences in economic and technological development for different areas can be explained by differences in geography (which affects factors like population density and spread of new technology) and differences in available crops and domesticatable animals.<ref>[[Richard E. Nisbett|Richard Nisbett]] argues in his 2004 ''The Geography of Thought'' that some of these regional differences shaped lasting cultural traits, such as the collectivism required by East Asian rice [[irrigation]], compared with the individualism of [[Ancient Greece|ancient Greek]] herding, maritime mercantilism, and money crops wine and olive oil (pp. 34-35).</ref> However,psychologist John Philippe Rushton suggests these environmental differences may operate in part by [[natural selection|selecting]] for higher levels of IQ<ref>This theory is discussed by {{AYref|Jensen|1998b}} (pp. 435-437), {{AYref|Lynn|1991b}} and {{AYref|Rushton|2000}} in general and by both {{AYref|Wade|2006}} and [http://www.isteve.com/diamond.htm Steve Sailer] with respect to ''Guns, Germs, and Steel''. See [[Race and intelligence (Explanations)#Rushton's application of r-K theory]]. .. {{AYref|Voight et al.|2006}} state generally that "a number of recent studies have detected more signals of adaptation in non-African populations than in Africans, and some of those studies have conjectured that non-Africans might have experienced greater pressures to adapt to new environments than Africans have" ({{AYref|Kayser et al.|2003}}, {{AYref|Akey et al.|2004}}, {{AYref|Storz et al.|2004}}, {{AYref|Stajich and Hahn|2005}}, {{AYref|Carlson et al.|2005}}).</ref> There is no evidence to suggest that such selective forces occur in regards to IQ{{Fact|date=October 2007}}. Consensus at the [[American Psychological Association]] is that a partly genetic hypothesis is as of now, inadequate in explaining differences in IQ among population groups.<ref>{http://www.gifted.uconn.edu/siegle/research/Correlation/Intelligence.pdf Text of the APA consensus statement</ref>
=====Health=====
[[Image:Lead_levels_children.png|thumb|250px|right|Percentage of children aged 1-5 with blood lead levels ''at least'' 10 µg/dL. Black and hispanic children have much higher levels than white children. A 10 µg/dL increase in blood lead at 24 months is associated with a 5.8-point decline in IQ.<ref> [http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/cgi/content/abstract/90/6/855 Low-Level Lead Exposure, Intelligence and Academic Achievement: A Long-term Follow-up Study] David C. Bellinger PhD, MSc1, Karen M. Stiles PhD, MN1, and Herbert L. Needleman MD1. PEDIATRICS Vol. 90 No. 6 December 1992, pp. 855-861 </ref> In [[1976]] 77.8% of all children had ''at least'' this much lead in their blood.<ref>[http://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/mm5420a5.htm Blood Lead Levels --- United States, 1999--2002] CDC.</ref>]]
{{Main|Health and intelligence}}
{{seealso|Race and health}}
In the developing world there are are many factors can greatly decrease IQ scores. Examples include nutrition deficiencies in [[iodine]] and [[iron]]; certain diseases like [[malaria]]; unregulated toxic industrial substances like [[lead]] and [[mercury (element)|mercury]]; and poor health care for pregnant women and infants. Also in the developed world there are many biological factors that can affect IQ. Increased rates of low birth weight babies and lower rates of breastfeeding in Blacks as compared to Whites are some factors of many that have been proposed to affect the IQ gap.<ref name="seeHandI">Health</ref>


Other researchers have come across what they see as additional reasons for the IQ gap. The paper ''Poverty and Brain Development in Early Childhood'' holds that there is a large amount of neural damage in many American Black and Hispanic children due to inadequate nutrition, substance abuse of the children's parents, a high incidence of maternal depression, exposure to environmental toxins, psychological trauma, and the neural effects of physical abuse. {{A(Y)ref|Masters|1997}} has proposed a "neurotoxity hypothesis" where pre- and post-natal exposure to heavy metal poisons differentially impacts Blacks. Black children have much higher lead levels than white children.<ref>[http://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/mm5420a5.htm Blood Lead Levels --- United States, 1999--2002] CDC.</ref>Drug abuse during pregnancy (e.g., [[alcohol]] and [[phenobarbital]]) can negatively affect IQ.


A study of LBW babies indicates that breastfeeding can significantly improve their IQ scores tested at 8 years old ({{AYref|Lucas et al.|1996}}). After controlling for possible confounding factors, an improvement of 8.3 IQ points was reported in the breastfed group as compared to the formula fed group. Black mothers are known to breastfeed infants less and for a shorter time than White mothers ({{AYref|Ryan et al.|1996}}; {{AYref|Leary|1988}})<ref>[http://www.cdc.gov/MMWR/preview/mmwrhtml/mm5512a3.htm Racial and Socioeconomic Disparities in Breastfeeding --- United States, 2004] CDC</ref>Studies have shown IQ gains lasting into adulthood with increased duration of breastfeeding. Several recent studies shows that the intake of certain [[micronutrient]]s, like those present in breast milk or fish oil, affects IQ scores even in developed nations. {{A(Y)ref|Helland et al|2003}} have shown larger head size at birth and higher IQ scores at 4 years of age when mothers took fish oil supplements during pregnancy and lactation.<ref name="nutrition">"[http://www.pponline.co.uk/encyc/0020b.htm Fat, Fitness And Performance]," Peak Performance; I.B. Helland et al., "[http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&list_uids=12509593&dopt=Abstract Maternal supplementation with very-long-chain n-3 fatty acids during pregnancy and lactation augments children's IQ at 4 years of age]," ''Pediatrics'' 111, no. 1 (January 2003): 39&ndash;44.</ref> {{A(Y)ref|Jensen|1998}} believes that dietary supplementation is a promising avenue of research for raising Black children's levels of ''g''. {{A(Y)ref|Lynn|1990}} has proposed a nutritional hypothesis for the Flynn effect.


==Prejudice and IQ==
Exposure to violence in childhood has been associated with lower school grades<ref name="trauma">''[http://archpedi.ama-assn.org/cgi/content/abstract/156/3/280 Violence Exposure, Trauma, and IQ and/or Reading Deficits Among Urban Children]'' Virginia Delaney-Black, MD, MPH; Chandice Covington, PhD, RN, CPNP; Steven J. Ondersma, PhD; Beth Nordstrom-Klee, PhD; Thomas Templin, PhD; Joel Ager, PhD; James Janisse, PhD; Robert J. Sokol, MD Vol. 156 No. 3, March 2002</ref> and lower IQ in children of all races.<ref name="trauma2">[http://www.springerlink.com/content/p56w670331216805/ IQ and Posttraumatic Stress Symptoms in Children Exposed to Interpersonal Violence]</ref> A group of largely African American urban first-grade children and their caregivers were evaluated using self-report, interview, and standardized tests, including IQ tests. The study reported that exposure to violence and trauma-related distress in young children were associated with substantial decrements in IQ and reading achievement. Exposure to Violence or Trauma lead to a 7.5-point (SD, 0.5) decrement in IQ and a 9.8-point (SD, 0.66) decrement in reading achievement.<ref name="trauma">...</ref> Violence may have a negative impact on IQ, or IQ may be protective against violence.<ref name="trauma2"/> The causal mechanism and direction of causation is unknown.<ref name="trauma"/> Neighborhood risk has been related to lower school grades for African-American adolescents in another study from 2006.<ref>[http://www.springerlink.com/content/9387678780625375/ Family, peer, and neighborhood influences on academic achievement among African-American adolescents: One-year prospective effects]</ref>
A university study in the US was carried out in order to determine whether a relationship existed between prejudice and IQ. Students were given an IQ test and a test that measures racial prejudice. The study found that students who scored lower on IQ tests were more prejudiced. <ref>[http://www.amazon.com/gp/reader/0399529519/ How do you compare pages 28-29] ISBN 0399529519 based on Lapsley and Enright , ' The effect of social desirability, intelligence, and milieu on an American validation of the conservatism scale' </ref>


=====Culture=====
====
Many anthropologists have argued that intelligence is a cultural category; some cultures emphasize speed and competition more than others, for example. During WWI African-Americans from the north tested higher than those from the south. This could be because African-Americans in the north had received more formal education (see ''Race: Science and Politics'', written by Ruth Benedict in 1940). It is a matter of debate to what extent genetics can affect culture and what exactly are the causality relations between culture and nature.


'''Media portrayal of race and intelligence''' in various mediums, such as films, books, and newspapers, characterize people of various races to be more or less intelligent. Likewise, reporting on research into race and intelligence has been criticized: either for giving scientific theories of race too much credit, or for rejecting the theories of some researchers in the name of racial harmony.


===Examples===
<blockquote>It's been a personal challenge for Dylan Pritchett, a Lafayette High School senior in Williamsburg who will head to Old Dominion University in August. Friends accused him of not "acting black" when he signed up for [[AP courses]].<ref>[http://www.dailypress.com/news/dp-brown16,0,2855398.story?page=5 CLOSING THE GAP: Blacks and whites still show unequal academic achievement]</ref></blockquote>


Critics of contemporary media have highlighted portrayals of minorities as less intelligent<ref>[http://etd.lsu.edu/docs/available/etd-05282004-130909/unrestricted/Smith_thesis.pdf THE PORTRAYALS OF MINORITY CHARACTERS IN ENTERTAINING ANIMATED CHILDREN’S PROGRAMS]</ref> (or in the case of Asians, on occasion more intelligent<ref>[http://www.modelminority.com/article2.html Media Portrayals of Major League Baseball Pitchers]</ref>) in films and movies.
It has been suggested that Black culture disfavors academic achievement and fosters an environment that is damaging to IQ ({{AYref|Boykin|1994}}). Likewise, it is argued that a persistence of racism reinforces this negative effect. John Ogbu<ref name="Ogbu">{{AYref|Ogbu|1978}}; {{AYref|Ogbu|2002}}; {{AYref|Ogbu|2003}}</ref> has developed a hypothesis that the condition of being a "caste-like minority" affects motivation and achievement, depressing IQ.


======Language======
========
{{Seealso|African-American stereotypes}}
[[Sandra Lee McKay]] author of ''Sociolinguistics and Language Teaching'' writes that language may present a barrier for students who speak pidgin and creole languages. Unlike other languages such as [[Spanish language|Spanish]] and [[Chinese language|Chinese]], [[pidgin]] and creole languages such as [[African American Vernacular English]] (AAVE) are not commonly recognized in classroom settings. As a result of this, students are not taught the [[Standard American English]] (SAE) used on tests ''as a second language'' in the same way as students who speak [[Spanish language|Spanish]] or [[Chinese language|Chinese]]. Students who speak AAVE face challenges similar to those learning English as a Second Language. ([[ESL]])<ref>[http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/Home.portal?_nfpb=true&_pageLabel=RecordDetails&ERICExtSearch_SearchValue_0=EJ608154&ERICExtSearch_SearchType_0=eric_accno&objectId=0900000b8001f636 TOEFL to the Test: Are Monodialectal AAL-Speakes Similar to ESL Students?]</ref>
=====Early stereotypes=====
[[Image:Virginia Minstrels, 1843.jpg|thumb|150px|right|Early
[[minstrel shows]] lampooned the supposed stupidity of Blacks. Detail from cover of ''The Celebrated Negro Melodies, as Sung by the Virginia Minstrels'', 1843]]


Early [[minstrel shows]] lampooned the supposed stupidity of Blacks, movies such as [[Birth of a Nation]] questioned whether or not Black people were fit to run for governmental offices or vote. [[Secretary of State]] [[John C. Calhoun]] arguing for the extension of slavery in 1844 said ''"Here (scientific confirmation) is proof of the necessity of slavery. The African is incapable of self-care and sinks into lunacy under the burden of freedom. It is a mercy to give him the guardianship and protection from mental death."''
John Russel Rickford author of ''Unequal partnership: Sociolinguistics and the African American speech community''<ref>“Unequal partnership: Sociolinguistics and the African American speech community” John Russel Rickford. Language and Society 26/2 (1997), pp.161-97</ref> rebutts misconceptions about the cognitive limitations of the use of [[African American Vernacular English|AAVE]] notes the unfair disadvantages IQ tests pose for its speakers. Geneva Smitherman writes that "80 to 90 percent of American blacks” speak AAVE “at least some of the time".<ref>Geneva Smitherman, ''Talkin and Testifyin: The Language of Black America'' <!-- No, there's neither a "g" nor an apostrophe for the words in the main title --> (Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1977), 2.</ref> Anne H. Charity, Hollis S. Scarborough, Darion M. Griffin found in a 2004 study that higher familiarity with SE was associated with better reading achievement for urban African American students in kindergarten through second grade. The improvement in reading ability was independent of other cognitive measures suggesting that speakers of AAVE face barriers in education.<ref>''Familiarity With School English in African American Children and Its Relation to Early Reading Achievement'' Child Development 75 (5), 1340–1356. 2004</ref>


Even after slavery ended the intellectual capacity of Black people was still frequently questioned. Lewis Terman wrote in ''The measurement of intelligence'' in 1916 <blockquote>"(Black and other ethnic minority children) are uneducable beyond the nearest rudiments of training. No amount of school instruction will ever make them intelligent voters or capable citizens in the sense of the world…their dullness seems to be racial, or at least inherent in the family stock from which they come…Children of this group should be segregated in special classes and be given instruction which is concrete and practical. They cannot master abstractions, but they can be made efficient workers…There is no possibility at present of convincing society that they should not be allowed to reproduce, although from a eugenic point of view they constitute a grave problem because of their unusual prolific breeding."</blockquote>
======Role-model effects======
Thomas S. Dee, in his study''Teachers, Race, and Student Achievement in a Randomized Experiment'' found that the race of the teacher has impacts on student achievement. An own-race teacher significantly increased the math and reading achievement of both black and white students.<ref>[http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/abs/10.1162/003465304323023750?journalCode=rest Teachers, Race, and Student Achievement in a Randomized Experiment] Thomas S. Dee. The Review of Economics and Statistics. February 2004</ref> Using single-equation regression models Mark O. Evans has also found evidence of effects for African-American students. <ref>An Estimate of Race and Gender Role-Model Effects in Teaching High School Mark O. Evans The Journal of Economic Education, Vol. 23, No. 3 (Summer, 1992), pp. 209-217 doi:10.2307/1183223</ref> These findings may confirm the suggestion for the aggressive recruitment of minority teachers are based on hypothesized role-model effects for minority students.


=====Modern stereotypes=====
In Sabrina Zirkel's longitudinal study of young adolescents students who reported having at least one race- and gender-matched role model performed better academically up to 24 months later, reported more achievement-oriented goals, enjoyed achievement-relevant activities to a greater degree, thought more about their futures, and looked up to adults rather than peers more often than did students without a race- and gender-matched role model. These effects held only for race- and gender-matched role models—not for non-matched role models.<ref>[http://www.blackwell-synergy.com/links/doi/10.1111/1467-9620.00166 Is There A Place for Me? Role Models and Academic Identity among White Students and Students of Color] Sabrina Zirkel Teachers College Record. Volume 104 Issue 2 Page 357 - March 2002 </ref>
{{Seealso|Acting white}}

[[Image:Jjportrait.jpg|150px|thumb|right|Some regard [[Jar Jar]] as a thinly veiled version of the type of portrayals used in [[Minstrel show|minstrelsy]] to lampoon the supposed stupidity of Black people.]]
====Theories on the intelligence of Ashkenazi Jews====
{{See also|Ashkenazi intelligence}}
A link between disease mutations specific to the [[Ashkenazi Jews]] and high IQ scores has been suggested by scientists at the University of Utah, who cite evidence that sphingolipid disorders promote the growth and interconnection of brain cells and that mutations in the DNA repair genes, involved in some Ashkenazic diseases, may also unleash growth of neurons. The researchers predict that these disease mutations will enhance IQ in heterozygotes. This prediction is based on evidence that selection pressure has increased the frequency of the disease mutations in the reproductively isolated Ashkenazi population in medieval times. The hypothesis has not yet been empirically tested.<ref>http://homepage.mac.com/harpend/.Public/AshkenaziIQ.jbiosocsci.pdf</ref> Ashkenazi Jews have been reported to score 0.75 to 1.0 standard deviations above the general European average, corresponding to an IQ 112-115.<ref>[http://homepage.mac.com/harpend/.Public/AshkenaziIQ.jbiosocsci.pdf]</ref><ref>http://www.astarshop.com/j_dis.pdf</ref><ref>[http://www.economist.com/science/displaystory.cfm?story_id=E1_QDPNGPR]</ref>

====Genetic explanation====
{{seealso|Inheritance of intelligence}}

[[Image:Heritability plants.jpeg|thumb|left|250px|The height of this "ordinary genetically varied corn" is 100% heritable, but the difference between the groups is totally environmental.<ref>[http://www.nyu.edu/gsas/dept/philo/faculty/block/papers/Heritability.html How Heritability Misleads about Race]</ref>]]

[[Arthur Jensen]] and J. Philippe Rushton argue that the Black-White IQ gap is significantly genetic and reflects underlying group characteristics. That is, they argue that the same mix of genetic and environment factors that cause IQ differences among individuals or between families of the same race also causes the differences seen between races. In this view, the genetic contribution to average intelligence differences among races are like average skin color differences: a product of different allelic frequencies within each population. Others are critical of Jensen's methods and evaluation ({{AYref|Sternberg|2005}}; {{AYref|Suzuki and Aronson|2005}}; {{AYref|Nisbett|2005}}).

The results of most (indirect) analyses used to test the genetic hypothesis do not logically contradict a primarily environmental explanation of the lower IQ of Blacks. That is, a plausible (but some argue ''[[ad hoc]]'') environmental explanation for the lower mean IQ in Blacks can be offered in most cases.
[[Image:Jensen2.jpg|right|thumb|140px|Psychologist [[Arthur Jensen]] set off an enduring controversy with his 1969 article in the [[Harvard Educational Review]].]]
Arthur Jensen and J. Philippe Rushton have concluded that the US IQ gap is partially genetic. Rushton and Jensen say that while plausible environmental explanation for the lower mean IQ in Blacks in the U.S. can be offered in many cases, these explanations are less capable of explaining the higher average IQ of East Asians than Whites.

To support their theory, they often cite several arguments and observations:

# Black–White–East Asian differences in IQ, reaction time, and brain size are observed worldwide in a range of cultures and environments. In the United States, significant Black-White IQ differences are observable at every age above 3 years, within every occupation or socioeconomic level tested, in every region of the country, and at every time since the invention of ability tests.<ref>{{AYref|Jensen|1998b}}</ref>
# Jensen and J. Philippe Rushton have argued that the magnitude of race differences on different IQ subtests correlate with the extent to which those subtests measures ''g'',<ref>For example, see {{AYref|Rushton and Jensen|2003}}; see also [[Spearman's hypothesis]]</ref> which also correlates with measures of the subtests heritability.<ref>for example, [[inbreeding depression]] scores measured in Japan predict the magnitude of the Black-White gap in the United States. ({{AYref|Rushton|1989a}})</ref> From these and other findings, they argue that race differences have a partly biological basis.<ref>reviewed by {{AYref|Jensen|1998b}}</ref>
# The rising [[IQ#Development|heritability of IQ]] with age (within all races; studies have reported on average in the developed world heritability starts at 20% in infants, rises to 40% in middle childhood, and peaks at 80% in adulthood); and studies showing the virtual disappearance (~0.0) by adulthood of shared environmental effects on IQ (for example, family income, education, and home environment), with adopted siblings partaking in the studies no more similar in IQ than with strangers<ref>{{AYref|Plomin et al.|2001}}</ref> From these studies, they argue that most suggested environmental explanations for IQ difference between groups do not have a strong enough effect on IQ to fully account for group differences.
# Studies of US comparisons of both parents to children and siblings to each other finding [[Regression toward the mean|regression]] to differing means for different races (85 for Blacks and 100 for Whites) across the entire range of IQs,<ref>for example, the children of wealthy, high IQ Black parents score lower than the children of poor, low IQ White parents ({{AYref|Jensen|1998b}}, p. 358); and for Black and White children with an IQ of 120, the siblings of the Black children average an IQ of 100 whereas the siblings of the White children average an IQ of 110; in comparison, for Black and White children with an IQ of 70, the siblings of the Black children average an IQ of 78 whereas the siblings of the White children average an IQ of 85 ({{AYref|Jensen|1973}}, pp. 107–119))</ref> despite the fact that siblings are matched for shared environment and genetic heritage, with regression unaffected by family socioeconomic status and generation examined<ref>http://www.lrainc.com/swtaboo/taboos/cmurraybga0799.pdf</ref>
#Evidence against test construction and cultural bias: the internal consistency of item difficulty for all groups, the equivalent validity of tests in predicting academic and occupational outcomes for all groups, and the persistence of the IQ gap on relatively culture-free tests.<ref>{{AYref|Jensen|1980}}</ref>

{{A(Y)ref|Rushton and Jensen|2005a}} believe that the best explanation is that 50%-80% of the group differences in average US IQ is genetic.<ref>{{AYref|Rushton and Jensen|2005a}}, cited in "[http://www.eurekalert.org/pub_releases/2005-04/cdri-bai042505.php Black-White-East Asian IQ differences at least 50% genetic, scientists conclude in major law journal]", and {{AYref|Murray|2005}}</ref>

Other evidence, such as [[Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study|transracial adoption]], certain racial admixture studies, behavior genetic modeling of group differences, evolutionary explanations and "life-history" traits such as measures of maturation, personality, reproduction and social organization have also been proposed to indicate a genetic contribution to the IQ gaps and explain how these arose.<ref>Reviewed by {{AYref|Rushton and Jensen|2005}}.</ref> Critics of this view, such as [[Robert Sternberg]], argue that these studies are either flawed and thus inconclusive, or else that they support a primarily environment (<20% genetic) hypothesis.<ref>For example: {{AYref|Nisbett|2005}}, {{AYref|Suzuki and Aronson|2005}}, {{AYref|Sternberg|2005}}, {{AYref|Dickens|2005}}</ref> For example, {{AYref|Dolan and Hamaker|2001}} argue that the statistical methods linking the Black-White gap to ''g'' are insufficient.<ref>{{AYref|Dolan and Hamaker|2001}} reanalyzed the data from several earlier studies and concluded that Spearman's hypothesis is not an "empirically established fact" (i.e., that Black-White IQ differences may be due to differences in common factors other than ''g'') due to insufficient power in the data to choose between alternative models. "This leaves the validity of Spearman's hypothesis, considered a central justification for the genetic explanation, an unresolved question." However, they did confirm that the Black-White IQ gap is not due to measurement artifacts, and is instead due to some measured factor that varies both within and between groups.</ref>

According to [[Linda Gottfredson]], a researcher at the University of Delaware [[IQ]] differences among individuals of the same [[race]] reflect (1) real, (2) functionally/socially significant, and (3) substantially genetic differences in the [[general intelligence factor]] ({{AYref|Gottfredson|2005b}}, p. 311). Also, again according to Dr Gottfredson, average IQ differences among races reflect (1) real and (2) significant differences in the same ''g'' factor ({{AYref|Gottfredson|2005b}}, p. 311). However, it is a matter of debate whether IQ differences among races in a given [[country]] are primarily environmental, primarily genetic or simply an artifact of an inaccurate use of social racial identification as a proxy for genetics.<ref name=cooper>[http://www.apa.org/journals/releases/amp60171.pdf Race and IQ: Molecular Genetics as Deus ex Machina], Richard S. Cooper</ref>

A recent review summarizing the arguments for a genetic explanation can be found here.<ref>http://www.ssc.uwo.ca/psychology/faculty/rushtonpdfs/PPPL1.pdf</ref> A critique of genetic explanations can be found here.<ref>http://taxa.epi.umn.edu/~mbmiller/journals/pppl/200504/2/302-2.html</ref>

==Media portrayal==
Media portrayal of race and intelligence in various mediums, such as films, books, and newspapers, characterize people of various races to be more or less intelligent. Critics of contemporary media have highlighted portrayals of minorities as less intelligent<ref>[http://etd.lsu.edu/docs/available/etd-05282004-130909/unrestricted/Smith_thesis.pdf THE PORTRAYALS OF MINORITY CHARACTERS IN ENTERTAINING ANIMATED CHILDREN’S PROGRAMS]</ref> (or in the case of Asians, on occasion more intelligent<ref>[http://www.modelminority.com/article2.html Media Portrayals of Major League Baseball Pitchers]</ref>) in films and movies.


Patricia J. Williams, writer for [[The Nation]], said this of [[Jar Jar]] Binks, a character from the 2002 [[Star Wars]] film: "...intentionally or not, Jar Jar's pratfalls and high jinks borrow heavily from the genre of minstrelsy. Despite the amphibian get-up, his relentless, panicky, manchild-like idiocy is imported directly from the days of [[Amos 'n' Andy|Amos 'N' Andy]]." Many aspects of Jar Jar's character are believed to be highly reminiscent of the archetypes portrayed in [[blackface]] [[Minstrel show|minstrelsy]].<ref
Patricia J. Williams, writer for [[The Nation]], said this of [[Jar Jar]] Binks, a character from the 2002 [[Star Wars]] film: "...intentionally or not, Jar Jar's pratfalls and high jinks borrow heavily from the genre of minstrelsy. Despite the amphibian get-up, his relentless, panicky, manchild-like idiocy is imported directly from the days of [[Amos 'n' Andy|Amos 'N' Andy]]." Many aspects of Jar Jar's character are believed to be highly reminiscent of the archetypes portrayed in [[blackface]] [[Minstrel show|minstrelsy]].<ref
Line 427: Line 411:
| accessdate=June 11
| accessdate=June 11
| accessyear=2006
| accessyear=2006
|date=June 17, 1999
|date=June 17, 1999
}}
}}
</ref>
</ref>
Line 436: Line 420:


Even so-called positive images of Black people can lead to stereotypes about intelligence. In ''Darwin's Athletes: how sport has damaged Black America and preserved the myth of race'', [[John Hoberman]] writes that the prominence of African-American athletes encourages a de-emphasis on academic achievement in black communities.<ref>''Darwin's Athletes: how sport has damaged Black America and preserved the myth of race'' By [[John Hoberman|John Milton Hoberman]] ISBN 0395822920</ref> In a 1997 study on racial stereotypes in sports, participants were shown a photograph of a white or a black basketball player. They then listened to a recorded radio broadcast of a basketball game. White photographs were rated as exhibiting significantly more intelligence in the way they played the game, even though the radio broadcast and target player represented by the photograph were the same throughout the trial.<ref>''"White Men Can't Jump": Evidence for the Perceptual Confirmation of Racial Stereotypes Following a Basketball Game'' Jeff Stone, ‌W. Perry, ‌John M. Darley. Basic and Applied Social Psychology 1997, Vol. 19, No. 3, Pages 291-306</ref> Several other authors have said that sports coverage that highlights 'natural black athleticism' has the effect of suggesting white superiority in other areas, such as intelligence.<ref>''The Ball Curve: Calculated Racism and the Stereotype of African American Men'' Ronald E. Hall Journal of Black Studies, Vol. 32, No. 1 (Sep., 2001), pp. 104-119</ref>
Even so-called positive images of Black people can lead to stereotypes about intelligence. In ''Darwin's Athletes: how sport has damaged Black America and preserved the myth of race'', [[John Hoberman]] writes that the prominence of African-American athletes encourages a de-emphasis on academic achievement in black communities.<ref>''Darwin's Athletes: how sport has damaged Black America and preserved the myth of race'' By [[John Hoberman|John Milton Hoberman]] ISBN 0395822920</ref> In a 1997 study on racial stereotypes in sports, participants were shown a photograph of a white or a black basketball player. They then listened to a recorded radio broadcast of a basketball game. White photographs were rated as exhibiting significantly more intelligence in the way they played the game, even though the radio broadcast and target player represented by the photograph were the same throughout the trial.<ref>''"White Men Can't Jump": Evidence for the Perceptual Confirmation of Racial Stereotypes Following a Basketball Game'' Jeff Stone, ‌W. Perry, ‌John M. Darley. Basic and Applied Social Psychology 1997, Vol. 19, No. 3, Pages 291-306</ref> Several other authors have said that sports coverage that highlights 'natural black athleticism' has the effect of suggesting white superiority in other areas, such as intelligence.<ref>''The Ball Curve: Calculated Racism and the Stereotype of African American Men'' Ronald E. Hall Journal of Black Studies, Vol. 32, No. 1 (Sep., 2001), pp. 104-119</ref>

====East Asian intelligence stereotypes====
{{seealso|Media portrayal of East and Southeast Asians#Model minority}}
[[Image:The Doctor.jpg|thumb|left|80px|Fu Manchu.]]
Asians have generally been portrayed in the media as intelligent but unsociable.<ref>Katz/Braly(1933), Karlins, Coffman, & Walters, 1969; Maykovich, 1972</ref> The early 20th century fictional character [[Fu Manchu]] was one startling example of this kind of media portrayal:

{{cquote|Imagine a person, tall, lean and feline, high-shouldered, with a brow like [[Shakespeare]] and a face like [[Satan]], a close-shaven skull, and long, magnetic eyes of the true cat-green. Invest him with all the cruel cunning of an entire Eastern race, accumulated in one giant intellect, with all the resources of science past and present... Imagine that awful being, and you have a mental picture of Dr. Fu-Manchu, the [[yellow peril]] incarnate in one man. –''The Insidious Dr. Fu Manchu''}}

====White intelligence stereotypes====
{{Seealso|Stereotypes of whites}}
[[Image:Irish-stereotypes.jpg|thumb|right|200px|The cartoon above (New Physiognomy, New York, 1866), contrasts Florence Nightingale, the Civil War nurse, with "Bridget McBruiser", the stereotypical Irish woman.]]
[[Image:Scientific racism irish.gif|thumb|right|200px|Scientific Racism from an American magazine, [[Harper’s Weekly]], says that the Irish are similar to 'Negroes' and wonders why both groups are not extinct.]]

The social definition of "White" has changed over the years, and several White groups have at times been portrayed by the media as unintelligent. This includes ethnic groups such as the British, Irish, and Slavs.<ref>Leo W. Jeffres, K. Kyoon Hur (1979) White Ethnics and their Media Images Journal of Communication 29 (1), 116–122.</ref>

=====English intelligence stereotypes=====
The [[English people]] are stereotyped as inordinately proper, prudish, and stiff and as having bad teeth.<ref> [http://www.guardian.co.uk/notesandqueries/query/0,5753,-22429,00.html "A staple of American humor about the UK is the population's bad teeth."] </ref> Characters in historical movies often have English accents even when the setting has nothing to do with England. Upper-class characters are also often given English accents. In more recent times, many movie villains, including [[Jafar (Aladdin)|Jafar]] from ''[[Aladdin (1992 film)|Aladdin]]'', [[Scar (The Lion King)|Scar]] from ''[[The Lion King]]'', [[Hans Gruber]] from ''[[Die Hard]]'', and [[Hannibal Lecter]] from ''[[The Silence of the Lambs (film)|The Silence of the Lambs]]'', have all been portrayed by British actors or given English accents.

Notably, in Disney films from the 1990s onward, English accents are generally employed to serve one of two purposes: slapstick comedy or evil genius.<ref> [http://www.bbc.co.uk/dna/h2g2/A891155 "Why Villains in Movies Have English Accents".] January 15, 2003 </ref> Examples include ''Aladdin'' (the Sultan and Jafar, respectively), ''The Lion King'' ([[Zazu]] and Scar, respectively), ''[[The Hunchback of Notre Dame (1996 film)|The Hunchback of Notre Dame]]'' (Victor the Gargoyle and Frollo, respectively), and ''[[Pocahontas (1995 film)|Pocahontas]]'' (Wiggins and Ratcliffe, respectively, both of whom happen to be played by the same actor, American [[David Ogden Stiers]]).

These two stereotypes are compounded in a scene in ''Pocahontas'', in which Ratcliffe menacingly mentions giving the savages a "proper English greeting", in response to which Wiggins holds up two gift baskets.

=====Irish intelligence stereotypes=====
{{Seealso|Irish jokes}}
Although the Irish, Germans, French, etc are considered ethnic groups today, the common term in the 19th century was "race". Much was made of Celtic versus Anglo-Saxon racial characteristics, regarding historic identity and behavior patterns. An analysis of nineteenth-century British attitudes by [[Mary J. Hickman]] and Bronwen Walter wrote that the 'Irish Catholic' was one viewed as an "[[Other#The Other in the Social Sciences|other]]," or a different race in the construction of the British nationalist myth. Likewise the Irish considered the English "other" and fought hard to break away and create their own homeland, which they finally did in the 1920s. <ref>''Deconstructing Whiteness: Irish Women in Britain'' Mary J. Hickman, Bronwen Walter
''Feminist Review'', No. 50, The Irish Issue: The British Question (Summer, 1995), pp. 5-19 doi:10.2307/1395487</ref>

One 19th century British cartoonist even depicted [[Irish people|Irish]] immigrants as ape-like and as racially different. One American doctor in the 1850s [[James Redfield]], argued that "facial angle" was a sign of intelligence and character. He likened the facial characteristics of the human races to animals. Thus Irishmen resembled dogs, Yankees were like bears, Germans like lions, Negroes like elephants, Englishmen like bulls, Turks like turkeys, Persians like peacocks, Greeks like sheep, Hindus like swans, Jews like goats, and Frenchmen like frogs. [http://books.google.com/books?vid=ISBN0766179842&id=qJW5iuEKdCMC&pg=PA13&lpg=PA13&ots=8CX0kmAm4f&dq=James+Redfield+1852+Comparative+physiognomy&sig=uYg9nTMRiF1XF1w7m37elYwKyZ0#PPA11,M1] In the 20th century physical stereotypes survived in the comic books until the 1950s, with Irish characters like Mutt and Jeff, and Jiggs and Maggie appearing daily in hundreds of newspapers. <ref> Kerry Soper, "Performing 'Jiggs': Irish Caricature and Comedic Ambivalence toward Asøsimilation and the American Dream in George McManus's Bringing Up Father." ''Journal of the Gilded Age and Progressive Era'' 4.2 (2005): 72 pars. 30 Mar. 2007 [http://www.historycooperative.org/cgi-bin/justtop.cgi?act=justtop&url=http://www.historycooperative.org/journals/jga/4.2/soper.html online]. </ref>

=====Jewish intelligence stereotypes=====
{{Seealso|Racial antisemitism}}
Modern European antisemitism has its origin in 19th century theories—now mostly considered as [[pseudo-science|pseudo-scientific]]—that said that the Semitic peoples, including the Jews, are entirely different from the [[Aryan]], or [[Proto-Indo-Europeans|Indo-European]], populations, and that they can never be amalgamated with them. In this view, Jews are not opposed on account of their [[religion]], but on account of their supposed hereditary or genetic [[racial characteristics]] including: greed, a special aptitude for money-making and low cunning.

In early films such as Cohen's Advertising Scheme (1904, silent) stereotyped Jews as "scheming merchants"<ref>[http://www.lib.berkeley.edu/MRC/imagesjews.html The Movies, Race, and Ethnicity: Jews]</ref>

To this day Jewish people are sometimes stereotyped in media as being intellectually gifted.<ref>[http://www.interfaithfamily.com/site/apps/nl/content2.asp?c=ekLSK5MLIrG&b=297398&ct=3525311 Not Crazy About Goy Crazy] By Lynn Melnick</ref>

===Portrayal of research by the media===
====The Bell Curve====
{{main|The Bell Curve}}
[[Image:Charles Murray.gif|right|thumbnail|127px|[[Charles Murray (author)|Charles Murray]]]]

The Bell Curve is a controversial, best-selling 1994 book by [[Richard J. Herrnstein]] and [[Charles Murray]] exploring the role of intelligence in American life. The book became widely read and debated due to its discussion of [[race and intelligence]] in Chapters 13 and 14.

Press coverage has given considerable positive attention to theories of genetic racial differences in intelligence even though there is no consensus among researchers regarding their validity.<ref>[http://taxa.epi.umn.edu/~mbmiller/journals/pppl/200504/2/302-2.html HEREDITY, ENVIRONMENT, AND RACE DIFFERENCES IN IQ: A Commentary on Rushton and Jensen (2005)] Richard E. Nisbett Psychology, Public Policy, and Law June 2005 Vol. 11, No. 2, 302-310</ref> Upon publication, ''[[The Bell Curve]]'' received a great deal of positive publicity, including cover stories in ''Newsweek'' ("the science behind [it] is overwhelmingly mainstream"), early publication (under protest by other writers and editors) by ''The New Republic'' by its editor-in-chief at the time [[Andrew Sullivan]], and ''The New York Times Book Review'' (which suggested critics disliked its "appeal to sweet reason" and are "inclined to hang the defendants without a trial"). Early articles and editorials appeared in ''Time'', ''The New York Times'' ("makes a strong case"), ''The New York Times Magazine'', ''Forbes'', the ''Wall Street Journal'', and ''National Review''. It received a respectful airing on such shows as ''Nightline'', the ''MacNeil/Lehrer NewsHour'', the ''McLaughlin Group'', ''Think Tank'', ''PrimeTime Live'', and ''All Things Considered''. [http://www.fair.org/extra/9501/bell.html]

The positive reception of ''The Bell Curve'' in media such as newspapers and television talk shows was troubling to critics such as economist [[Edward S. Herman]] and evolutionary biologist [[Joseph L. Graves]] who felt that it indicated a troubling acceptance of what Herman calls deterministic racist doctrines.<ref>[http://www.zmag.org/zmag/articles/herman1.htm Fog Watch: The New Racist Onslaught]</ref> Dennis M. Rutledge suggests that through [[soundbite]]s of works like Jensen's famous study on the achievement gap, and Herrnstein and Murray's book ''The Bell Curve,'' the media "''paints a picture of Blacks and other people of color as collective biological illiterates — as not only intellectually unfit but evil and criminal as well,"'' thus providing, he says ''"the logic and justification for those who would further disenfranchise and exclude racial and ethnic minorities."''<ref>[http://www.findarticles.com/p/articles/mi_qa3626/is_199507/ai_n8730395 Social Darwinism, scientific racism, and the metaphysics of race] Journal of Negro Education, The, Summer 1995 by Dennis, Rutledge M</ref>

=====APA response=====
{{main|The Bell Curve}}
In response to the controversy surrounding ''[[The Bell Curve]]'', the [[American Psychological Association]]'s Board of Scientific Affairs in [[1995]] established a special task force to publish an investigative report on the research presented in the book. [http://www.lrainc.com/swtaboo/taboos/apa_01.html Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns]. Regarding genetic causes, they judged that there is not much direct evidence on this point, but what little there is fails to support the genetic hypothesis. The January 1997 issue of ''American Psychologist'' included eleven critical responses to the APA report, most of which criticized the report's failure to examine all of the evidence for or against the partly-genetic interpretation of racial differences in IQ.{{Fact|date=January 2007}} <!--please add footnote, ideally with a quote from the text-->

==== Stereotype threat ====
{{main|Stereotype threat}}
Stereotype threat is the fear that one's behavior will confirm an existing [[stereotype]] of a group with which one identifies. This fear may in turn lead to an impairment of performance (Aronson, Wilson, & Akert, 2005). Stereotype threat was first articulated and documented by the social psychologists Claude Steele, Joshua Aronson, and Steven Spencer, who have conducted several studies on this topic.

While Stereotype threat has not received as much media attention as ''[[The Bell Curve]]'' much of the media coverage has been positive. ''[[The Atlantic Monthly]]'' ran a feature article on the topic authored by psychologist [[Claude M. Steele]] in August 1999.<ref>[http://www.theatlantic.com/doc/prem/199908/student-stereotype Thin Ice: Stereotype Threat and Black College Students] by Claude M. Steele</ref> Still one conservative researcher feels that the coverage has been inaccurate. In a [[2004]] study Sackett said he found indications of widespread and systematic research misinterpretation regarding one of the more popular explanations for the IQ gap.<ref>{{AYref|Sackett et al.|2004}}: "One [issue raised by readers of this article] is that misinterpretation of research is regrettably all too common and thus that documenting misinterpretations in one single domain is of limited interest. Our response is that we are singling out this domain because the issue at stake is of such importance and because the interpretive errors are so rampant and so systematic" (p. 11).</ref> Introducing ''stereotype threat'' to a test-taking environment has been shown to increase the existing gap between Blacks or Whites in relation to Whites or Asians respectively, and has thus been offered as a potential contributor to the gap.<ref>Other researchers have extended these results to other groups (e.g., gender, age) (p. 11).</ref> However Sackett said 88% of accounts in the popular media, 91% in scientific journals, and 67% in psychology textbooks had misinterpreted the findings as that eliminating the introduced stereotype threat eliminated the Black-White gap, when in fact the students had already been matched according to prior scores.<ref>pp. 10-11.</ref> Sackett suggests the appeal of the misinterpreted findings may have been a factor, and that such research results in general may in this way be systemically more readily accepted.<ref>"We can only speculate as to causes of the mischaracterization of the Steele and Aronson (1995) findings in these various media. . . A factor contributing to not noticing the adjustment may be the appeal of the misinterpreted findings (i.e., the conclusion that eliminating stereotype threat eliminates African American–White differences). Finding mechanisms to reduce or eliminate subgroup differences is an outcome that we believe would be virtually universally welcomed. Thus, research findings that can be interpreted as contributing to that outcome may be more readily accepted with less critical scrutiny" (p. 11).</ref>

====Snyderman and Rothman====
{{Main|Snyderman and Rothman (study)}}

The Snyderman and Rothman study accused the media of liberal bias in reporting on race and intelligence. Mark Snyderman and Stanley Rothman argued in their joint paper in 1988 that media coverage of intelligence-related research is often inaccurate and misleading. They surveyed the opinions of journalists and science editors and intelligence experts (not necessarily with knowledge about race), including scholars in the subfields of psychology, sociology, cognitive science, education, and genetics. They argue that media coverage of intelligence related topics was overall inaccurate and misleading. They say the media has misreported the views of the scientific community, especially about the role of genetic and environmental factors in explaining individual and group differences in IQ.

In their 1987 survey, they wrote:
<blockquote>''Forty-five percent believe the difference to be a product of both genetic and environmental variation, compared to only 15% who feel the difference is entirely due to environmental variation. Twenty-four percent of experts do not believe there are sufficient data to support any reasonable opinion, and 14% did not respond to the question. Eight experts (1%) indicate a belief in an entirely genetic determination.''</blockquote>

No poll option was provided to indicate "predominantly (but not entirely) environmental.

They found that the media regularly presented the views of [[Stephen Jay Gould]] and [[Leon Kamin]] as representative of mainstream opinion among experts, whereas those who stress that individual and group differences may be substantially genetic (e.g., [[Arthur Jensen]]) are characterized as a minority. According to Synderman and Rothman, their survey of expert opinion found that the opposite is true, however proportion of experts supporting these hypotheses today is unknown.

====Surveys of academic opinion====
A survey was conducted in 1987 of a broad sample of 1,020 scholars in specialties that would give them reason to be knowledgeable about IQ (but not necessarily about race). The survey was given to members of the [[American Education Research Association]], [[National Council on Measurement in Education]], [[American Psychological Association]], [[American Sociological Association]], [[Behavior Genetics Association]], and [[Cognitive Science Society]]. According to the report, regarding the question "The source of black-white difference in IQ":

<blockquote>This is perhaps the central question in the IQ controversy. Respondents were asked to express their opinion of the role of genetic differences in the black-white IQ differential. Forty-five percent believe the difference to be a product of both genetic and environmental variation, compared to only 15% who feel the difference is entirely due to environmental variation. Twenty-four percent of experts do not believe there are sufficient data to support any reasonable opinion, and 14% did not respond to the question. Eight experts (1%) indicate a belief in an entirely genetic determination.<ref>{{AYref|Snyderman and Rothman|1987}}.</ref></blockquote>

[[Robert Sternberg]] cautioned against supposing that the survey represented anything but opinion saying, "science isn't done by majority rule".<ref> (1995) [http://www.uclan.ac.uk/facs/science/psychol/Psychology/Skeptic.htm]</ref> Respondents on average called themselves slightly left of center politically, but political and social opinions accounted for less than 10% of the variation in responses. Carol Swain, author of ''The New White Nationalism'' reacted with some dismay to the survey, stating:

<blockquote>At least one important survey suggests that a belief in the biological inferiority of some races in regard to intelligence is more common than generally supposed. Smith College professor Stanley Rothman and Harvard researcher Mark Snyderman surveyed a sample of mostly scientific experts in the field of educational psychology in the late 1980s and found that 53 percent believed IQ differences between whites and African Americans were at least partly genetic in origin, while only 17 percent attributed the IQ differences to environmental factors alone (the remainder either believed the data was currently insufficient to decide the issue or refused to answer the question).</blockquote>

According to the [[American Psychological Association]]'s [[Race and intelligence#Collective Statements|1995 task force report]] on intelligence research:

<blockquote>It is sometimes suggested that the Black/White differential in psychometric intelligence is partly due to genetic differences (Jensen, 1972). There is not much direct evidence on this point, but what little there is fails to support the genetic hypothesis.<ref name="APA-report"/></blockquote>

The APA subsequently published eleven critical responses in 1997, most arguing that the report failed to examine adequately the evidence for the genetic hypothesis.<ref name=mackenzie/><ref>(''[[American Psychologist]]'', January 1997)</ref> [[Charles Murray (author)|Charles Murray]], for instance, responded:<blockquote>Actually, there is no direct evidence at all, just a wide variety of indirect evidence, almost all of which the task force chose to ignore.<ref>Murray lists race differences in brain size, along with "IQ in sub-Saharan Africa, the results of transracial adoption studies, the correlation of the black-white difference with the g-loadedness of tests, regression to racial means across the range of IQ, or other relevant data" among the arguments omitted from the task force report.[http://www.commentarymagazine.com/production/files/murray0905.html#_ednref50]</ref></blockquote>
The report did agree with many of the non-race-based statements on intelligence made in ''The Bell Curve''<ref>The authors of the report agreed that IQ scores have high predictive validity for individual differences in school achievement. They confirmed the predictive validity of IQ for adult occupational status, even when variables such as education and family background have been statistically controlled. They agree that individual differences in intelligence are substantially influenced by genetics (75% in adults). Consistent with Herrnstein and Murray's findings, they state there is little evidence to show that childhood diet influences intelligence except in cases of severe malnutrition.</ref> and concludes with a call for more reflection in debates on intelligence and for a "shared and sustained effort" in more research to answer the many unanswered questions that remain.<ref>"In a field where so many issues are unresolved and so many questions unanswered, the confident tone that has characterized most of the debate on these topics is clearly out of place. The study of intelligence does not need politicized assertions and recriminations; it needs self-restraint, reflection, and a great deal more research. The questions that remain are socially as well as scientifically important. There is no reason to think them unanswerable, but finding the answers will require a shared and sustained effort as well as the commitment of substantial scientific resources. Just such a commitment is what we strongly recommend."</ref>
Coming advances in [[genetics]] and [[genomics]] are expected to soon provide the ability to test hypotheses about group differences more rigorously than has as yet been possible.<ref>{{AYref|Pinker|2006}}, {{AYref|Rowe|2005}}, {{AYref|Stock|2002}} pp. 44-47.</ref>

Researchers who believe that there is no significant genetic contribution to race differences in intelligence include {{AYref|Flynn|1980}}, {{AYref|Brody|1992}}, {{AYref|Neisser et al.|1996}}, {{AYref|Nisbett|1998}}, {{AYref|Mackintosh|1998}}, {{AYref|Jencks and Phillips|1998}}, and {{AYref|Fish|2002}}. Some scientists who emphasize cultural explanations do not necessarily exclude a small genetic influence. {{A(Y)ref|Reynolds|2000}} suggests up to 20% genetic influence be included in the cultural explanation. Researchers who believe that there are significant genetic contributions to race differences in intelligence include {{AYref|McGurk|1953}}, {{AYref|Garrett|1961}}, {{AYref|Shuey|1966}}, {{AYref|Shockley|1968}}, {{AYref|Eysenck|1971}}, {{AYref|Baker|1974}}, {{AYref|Loehlin et al.|1975}}, {{AYref|Vernon|1979}}, {{AYref|Lynn|1991a}}, {{AYref|Waldman et al.|1994}}, {{AYref|Scarr|1995}}, {{AYref|Levin|1997}}, {{AYref|Jensen|1998b}}, {{AYref|Rushton|2000}}, and {{AYref|Gottfredson|2005b}}.

==== Opinions of scholars and others ====

A survey was conducted in 1987 of a broad sample of 1,020 scholars (65% replied) in specialties that would give them reason to be knowledgeable about IQ (but not necessarily about race; Snyderman & Rothman, 1987). The survey was given to members of the [[American Education Research Association]], [[National Council on Measurement in Education]], [[American Psychological Association]], [[American Sociological Association]], [[Behavior Genetics Association]], and [[Cognitive Science Society]]. Political and social opinions, reported in the same survey, accounted for less than 10% of the variation in responses. (Respondents on average called themselves slightly left of center politically.) Measures of expertise or eminence accounted for little or no variation in responses.

One question was "Which of the following best characterizes your ''opinion'' of the heritability of the Black-White difference in I.Q.?" (emphasis original).<ref name="apologia">Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray, "[http://www.mugu.com/cgi-bin/Upstream/atoms/xtra1/herrnstein-murray-tnr.html Race, genes and I.Q.&mdash;an apologia: the case for conservative multiculturalism]," ''The New Republic'' 211, no. 11 (October 1994): 27.</ref> The responses were divided into five categories:
* The difference is entirely due to environmental variation: 15%.
* The difference is entirely due to genetic variation: 1% (8 respondents).
* The difference is a product of both genetic and environmental variation: 45%.
* The data are insufficient to support any reasonable opinion: 24%.
* No response (or not qualified): 14%.

{| class="wikitable"
|+ A selection of survey results
!Question
!Responses
|-
| What heritability would you estimate for IQ differences within the White population? || Average estimate of 60 (± 17) percent.
|-
| What heritability would you estimate for IQ differences within the Black population? || Average estimate of 57 (± 18) percent.
|-
| Are intelligence tests biased against Blacks? || On a scale of 1 (not at all or insignificantly) to 4 (extremely), mean response of 2 (somewhat).
|-
| What is the source of the average Black-White difference in IQ? || Both genetic and environmental (45%, or 52% of those responding).
|}

The age of the survey and the anonymity of the respondents could constrain its interpretation.

In a 1988 survey, journalists, editors, and IQ experts were asked their "opinion of the source of the black-white difference in IQ" {{AYref|Snyderman and Rothman|1988}}
{| class="wikitable"
!Group
!Entirely Environment
!Entirely Genetic
!Both
!Data Are Insufficient
|-
| Journalists || 34% || 1% || 27% || 38%
|-
| Editors || 47% || 2% || 23% || 28%
|-
| IQ Experts || 17% || 1% || 53% || 28%
|}

===== The view of the American Psychological Association =====
In response to the controversy surrounding ''The Bell Curve'', the [[American Psychological Association]]'s Board of Scientific Affairs in [[1995]] established a special task force to publish an investigative report on the research presented in the book.<ref>http://www.lrainc.com/swtaboo/taboos/apa_01.html</ref>

The task force agrees that there do exist large differences between the average IQ scores of blacks and whites, and that these differences cannot be attributed to biases in test construction, nor does it "simply reflect differences in socio-economic status". While they admit there is no empirical evidence supporting it, the APA task force suggests that explanations based on social status and cultural differences may be possible. Regarding genetic causes, they noted, "There is not much direct evidence on this point, but what little there is fails to support the genetic hypothesis." The January 1997 issue of ''American Psychologist'' included eleven critical responses to the APA report, most of which criticized the report's failure to examine all of the evidence for or against the genetic hypothesis of racial differences in IQ.


== Controversies ==
== Controversies ==
{{main|Race and intelligence (Controversies)}}


===Utility of research===
===Utility of research===
{{Main|Race and intelligence (Utility of research)}}
Theories of race and intelligence have been challenged on grounds of their [[utility]]. Critics want to know what purpose such research could serve and why it has been an intense an area of focus for a few researchers. Some defend the research, saying it has egalitarian aims or that it is [[pure science]], others say that the true motivation for the research is the same as that of the [[eugenics]] movement and other forms of [[scientific racism]].<ref>e.g., Sternberg, 2003, pp. 386-387</ref><ref>e.g., Sternberg, 2003, pp. 386-387</ref> Even supporters of intelligence research have described such research as analogous to "working with dynamite" or "dangerous play" in sports.<ref>Hunt & Carlson, in press</ref>


As to whether research in this area is desirable, [[John C. Loehlin]] wrote in 1992, "Research on racial differences in intelligence is desirable ''if'' the research is appropriately motivated, honestly done, and adequately communicated." [emphasis original] Defenders of the research suggest that both scientific curiosity and a desire to draw benefits from the research are appropriate motivations. Researchers such as [[Richard Lynn]] have suggested that conclusions from the research can help make political decisions, such as the type of educational opportunities and expectations of achievement policy makers should have for people of different races. [[Charles Murray]], a conservative political pundit at the [[American Enterprise Institute]] has used their conclusions to criticize social programs based on racial equality that fail, he claims, to recognize the realities of racial differences.
Theories of race and intelligence have been challenged on grounds of their utility. Critics want to know what purpose such research could serve and why it has been an intense an area of focus for a few researchers. Some defend the research, saying it has egalitarian aims or that it is [[pure science]], others say that the true motivation for the reserch is the same as that of the [[eugenics]] movement and other forms of [[scientific racism]].<ref>e.g., Sternberg, 2003, pp. 386-387</ref><ref>e.g., Sternberg, 2003, pp. 386-387</ref>. Even supporters of intelligence research have described such research as analogous to "working with dynamite" or "dangerous play" in sports<ref>Hunt & Carlson, in press</ref>.

In a book review [[J. Philippe Rushton]] called [[Richard Lynn]]'s and [[Tatu Vanhanen]]'s book ''[[IQ And Global Inequality]]'' which links [[GDP]] with the racial composition of nations as "the most important contribution to economic understanding since [[Adam Smith]]".<ref>http://www.vdare.com/rushton/061207_iq.htm</ref>

Sociologist and demographer Reanne Frank says that some race and intelligence research has been abused "The most malignant are the "true believers," who subscribe to the typological distinctions that imply hierarchical rankings of worth across different races. Although this group remains small, the members' work is often widely publicized and well known (e.g., [[The Bell Curve|Herrnstein and Murray]] 1994; Rushton 1991)"<ref>Frank, Reanne, The Misuse of Biology in Demographic Research on Racial/Ethnic Differences: A Reply to van den Oord and Rowe, Demography - Volume 38, Number 4, November 2001, pp. 563-567</ref>

===Potential for bias===
{{main|Race and intelligence (Potential for bias)}}

Proponents of genetic explanations of race/IQ correlation have often been criticized both of [[scientific misconduct]] and of their intimate links with groups that have historic ties to Nazis and eugenics of the early 19th century, such as the [[Pioneer Fund]]. The [[Pioneer Fund]] has been characterized by the [[Southern Poverty Law Center]] as a [[hate group]]. Beverly Daniel Tatum writes that dominant cultures often set the parameters by which minority cultures will be judged. Minority groups are labeled as substandard in significant ways, for example blacks have historically been characterized as less intelligent than whites. Tatum suggests that the ability to set these parameters is a form of [[White privilege (sociology)|white privilege]].<ref name="Tatum">{{cite book| first=Beverly Daniel |last=Tatum |authorlink=Beverly Daniel Tatum |year=1997 |title=Why are all the black kids sitting together in the cafeteria? And other conversations about race |location=New York |publisher=BasicBooks |isbn=9780465091270}}</ref> Proponents of genetic explanations of race/IQ correlation have in turn accused their critics of [[scientific misconduct|suppressing scientific debate]] in the name of political correctness. They claim harassment and interference with both their work and funding.

The preponderance of evidence indicates that IQ tests measuring [[general intelligence]] are crossculturally valid. There is little or no evidence of population-specific cultural effects apart from the obvious example of language bias.<ref>http://www.charlesdarwinresearch.org/PRSL2007.pdf</ref> For example, [[Robert Sternberg]] et al. found that the IQ of 12- to 15-year-old Kenyans predicted school grades at about the same level as they do in the West.<ref>Sternberg, R. J., Nokes, C., Geissler, P. W., Prince, R.,
Okatcha, F., Bundy, D. A. & Grigorenko, E. L. 2001 The
relationship between academic and practical intelligence:
a case study in Kenya. Intelligence 29, 401–418.</ref> IQ also predicted university performance equally well in African and non-African engineering students in South Africa in a 2004 study.<ref>Construct validity of Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices for African and non-African engineering students in South Africa.</ref> Salgado et al. (2003) demonstrated the international generalizability of general mental ability across 10 member countries of the [[European Community]] and differences in a nation’s culture, religion, language, socioeconomic level or employment legislation did not affect the predictive validity of IQ tests.<ref>Salgado, J. F., Anderson, N., Moscoso, S., Bertua, C. &
Fruyt, F. D. 2003 International validity generalization of
GMA and cognitive abilities: a European community
meta-analysis. Pers. Psychol. 56, 573–605.</ref>

[[The Bell Curve]] has often been argued to embellish the view that IQ is inheritable. (Nature argument.) However, recent studies have argued that IQ itself is in fact malleable due to conditions of nuture.
<ref>Hawkes, N. (2007) 'Is there any truth in the claim that black people are less intelligent than whites?' The Times (Accessed Saturday October 20th 2007)</ref>


===Policy implications===
===Policy implications===
Line 447: Line 589:
Public policy implications of IQ and race research are one of the greatest sources of controversy surrounding this issue. Regardless of the source of the gap, most educators agree that it must be addressed. They often advocate equitable funding for education.<ref>[http://www.kirwaninstitute.org/publications/presentations/2006_09_23%20Battle%20Creek%20Education.ppt Achieving Equitable Education in Calhoun County]</ref><ref>[http://www.leaonline.com/doi/abs/10.1207/s15326993es4001_5 Educational Studies: A Jrnl of the American Educ. Studies Assoc.]</ref>
Public policy implications of IQ and race research are one of the greatest sources of controversy surrounding this issue. Regardless of the source of the gap, most educators agree that it must be addressed. They often advocate equitable funding for education.<ref>[http://www.kirwaninstitute.org/publications/presentations/2006_09_23%20Battle%20Creek%20Education.ppt Achieving Equitable Education in Calhoun County]</ref><ref>[http://www.leaonline.com/doi/abs/10.1207/s15326993es4001_5 Educational Studies: A Jrnl of the American Educ. Studies Assoc.]</ref>


Some proponents of a genetic<ref name=mackenzie/> interpretation of the IQ gap, such as {{A(Y)ref|Rushton and Jensen|2005a}} and {{A(Y)ref|Gottfredson|2005b}}, have sometimes argued that their interpretation does not in itself demand any particular policy response: while a conservative/[[Libertarianism|libertarian]] commentator<ref> For example, the policy recommendations of ''[[The Bell Curve]]'' were denounced by many.{{Fact|date=February 2007}} {{AYref|Herrnstein and Murray|1994}} wrote: "We can imagine no recommendation for using the government to manipulate fertility that does not have dangers. But this highlights the problem: The United States already has policies that inadvertently social-engineer who has babies, and it is encouraging the wrong women. ''If the United States did as much to encourage high-IQ women to have babies as it now does to encourage low-IQ women, it would rightly be described as engaging in aggressive manipulation of fertility.'' The technically precise description of America's fertility policy is that it subsidizes births among poor women, who are also disproportionately at the low end of the intelligence distribution. We urge generally that these policies, represented by the extensive network of cash and services for low-income women who have babies, be ended. (p. 548)"
Some proponents of a genetic interpretation of the IQ gap, such as {{A(Y)ref|Rushton and Jensen|2005a}} and {{A(Y)ref|Gottfredson|2005b}}, have sometimes argued that their interpretation does not in itself demand any particular policy response: while a conservative/[[Libertarianism|libertarian]] commentator<ref> For example, the policy recommendations of ''[[The Bell Curve]]'' were denounced by many.{{Fact|date=February 2007}} {{AYref|Herrnstein and Murray|1994}} wrote: "We can imagine no recommendation for using the government to manipulate fertility that does not have dangers. But this highlights the problem: The United States already has policies that inadvertently social-engineer who has babies, and it is encouraging the wrong women. ''If the United States did as much to encourage high-IQ women to have babies as it now does to encourage low-IQ women, it would rightly be described as engaging in aggressive manipulation of fertility.'' The technically precise description of America's fertility policy is that it subsidizes births among poor women, who are also disproportionately at the low end of the intelligence distribution. We urge generally that these policies, represented by the extensive network of cash and services for low-income women who have babies, be ended. (p. 548)"


Two year later the [[Personal Responsibility and Work Opportunity Act|1996 U.S. welfare reform]] substantially cut these programs. In a discussion of the future political outcomes of an intellectually stratified society, they stated that they: "fear that a new kind of conservatism is becoming the dominant ideology of the affluent - not in the social tradition of an [[Edmund Burke]] or in the economic tradition of an [[Adam Smith]] but ’conservatism’ along Latin American lines, where to be conservative has often meant doing whatever is necessary to preserve the mansions on the hills from the menace of the slums below. (p. 518)"Moreover, they fear that an increasing welfare will create a "custodial state": "a high-tech and more lavish version of the [[Indian reservation]] of some substantial minority of the nation’s population. They also predict increasing totalitarianism: It is difficult to imagine the United States preserving its heritage of individualism, equal rights before the law, free people running their own lives, once it is accepted that a significant part of the population must be made permanent wards of the states. (p. 526)"</ref> may feel the results justify, for example, reductions in [[affirmative action]], a [[Liberalism|liberal]] commentator may argue from a [[John Rawls|Rawlsian]] point of view (that genetic advantages are undeserved and unjust) for substantial affirmative action.<ref>{{AYref|Gottfredson|2005b}}</ref> Since all races have representatives at all levels of the IQ curve, this means any policy based on low IQ affects members of all races.
Two year later the [[Personal Responsibility and Work Opportunity Act|1996 U.S. welfare reform]] substantially cut these programs. In a discussion of the future political outcomes of an intellectually stratified society, they stated that they: "fear that a new kind of conservatism is becoming the dominant ideology of the affluent - not in the social tradition of an [[Edmund Burke]] or in the economic tradition of an [[Adam Smith]] but ’conservatism’ along Latin American lines, where to be conservative has often meant doing whatever is necessary to preserve the mansions on the hills from the menace of the slums below. (p. 518)"Moreover, they fear that an increasing welfare will create a "custodial state": "a high-tech and more lavish version of the [[Indian reservation]] of some substantial minority of the nation’s population. They also predict increasing totalitarianism: It is difficult to imagine the United States preserving its heritage of individualism, equal rights before the law, free people running their own lives, once it is accepted that a significant part of the population must be made permanent wards of the states. (p. 526)"</ref> may feel the results justify, for example, reductions in [[affirmative action]], a [[Liberalism|liberal]] commentator may argue from a [[John Rawls|Rawlsian]] point of view (that genetic advantages are undeserved and unjust) for substantial affirmative action.<ref>{{AYref|Gottfredson|2005b}}</ref> Since all races have representatives at all levels of the IQ curve, this means any policy based on low IQ affects members of all races.


While not specifically race-related, policies focused on geographical regions or nations may have disproportionate influences on certain racial groups and on cognitive development. Differences in health care, nutrition, regulation of environmental toxins, and geographic distribution of diseases and control strategies between the developing world and developed nations have all been subjects of policies or policy recommendations (see [[Health and intelligence]]).
According to the "[[Mainstream Science on Intelligence]]" statement published in ''[[Intelligence (journal)|Intelligence]]'' in 1997:

<blockquote>The research findings neither dictate nor preclude any particular social policy, because they can never determine our goals. They can, however, help us estimate the likely success and side-effects of pursuing those goals via different means.<ref>{{AYref|Gottfredson|1997a}}</ref></blockquote>


[[Melvin Konner]], professor of [[anthropology]] and associate professor of [[psychiatry]] and [[neurology]] at [[Emory University]], called ''Bell Curve'' a "deliberate assault on efforts to improve the school performance of African-Americans". "This book presented strong evidence that genes play a role in intelligence but linked it to the unsupported claim that genes explain the small but consistent black-white difference in IQ. The juxtaposition of good argument with a bad one seemed politically motivated, and persuasive refutations soon appeared. Actually, African-Americans have excelled in virtually every enriched environment they have been placed in, most of which they were previously barred from, and this in only the first decade or two of improved but still not equal opportunity. It is likely that the real curves for the two races will one day be superimposable on each other, but this may require decades of change and different environments for different people. Claims about genetic potential are meaningless except in light of this requirement."<ref>''The Tangled Wing Biological Constraints on the Human Spirit'' by Melvin Konner, 2nd edition, p. 428</ref>
While not specifically race-related, policies focused on geographical regions or nations may have disproportionate influences on certain racial groups and on cognitive development. Differences in health care, nutrition, regulation of environmental toxins, and geographic distribution of diseases and control strategies between the developing world and developed nations have all been subjects of policies or policy recommendations (see [[Intelligence and public policy#Health and Nutrition|health and nutrition policies relating to intelligence]]).


Finally, [[germinal choice technology]] may one day be able to select or change directly [[allele]]s found to influence intelligence or racially identifying traits (such as skin color; see gene [[SLC24A5]]), making them susceptible to biotechnological intervention.<ref>[[Gregory Stock]] argues "current debates about whether some of the differences among ethnic and racial groups are cultural or biological will soon become irrelevant, given the coming [malleability of biological traits]" ({{AYref|Stock|2002}}, p. 194; race and intelligence discussed on pp. 44-47).</ref>
Finally, [[germinal choice technology]] may one day be able to select or change directly [[allele]]s found to influence intelligence or racially identifying traits (such as skin color; see gene [[SLC24A5]]), making them susceptible to biotechnological intervention.<ref>[[Gregory Stock]] argues "current debates about whether some of the differences among ethnic and racial groups are cultural or biological will soon become irrelevant, given the coming [malleability of biological traits]" ({{AYref|Stock|2002}}, p. 194; race and intelligence discussed on pp. 44-47).</ref>


==End material==
==End material==
Line 474: Line 614:
<references/></div>
<references/></div>


===References===

{{main|Race and intelligence (References)}}


===External links===
===External links===
Line 511: Line 652:
* [http://iqte.st/blog/?p=258 Black-White Gap in IQ Scores Closing, Study Finds]
* [http://iqte.st/blog/?p=258 Black-White Gap in IQ Scores Closing, Study Finds]
* [http://www.sanjeev.sabhlokcity.com/breakingfree.html Breaking Free of Nehru]by Sanjeev Sabhlok : Chapter 2, section 6 of this book proposes a model of IQ determination which is strongly impacted by the level of freedom (and dignity)
* [http://www.sanjeev.sabhlokcity.com/breakingfree.html Breaking Free of Nehru]by Sanjeev Sabhlok : Chapter 2, section 6 of this book proposes a model of IQ determination which is strongly impacted by the level of freedom (and dignity)
{{Race and intelligence}}

{{Race and sex differences}}
{{Race and sex differences}}



Revision as of 09:00, 3 November 2007

Template:Race and intelligence2

The study of race and intelligence is the controversial study of how human intellectual capacities may vary among the different population groups commonly known as races. This study seeks to identify and explain the differences in manifestations of intelligence (e.g. IQ testing results), as well as the underlying causes of such variance.

Theories about the possibility of a relationship between race and intelligence have been the subject of speculation and debate since the 16th century.[1][2] The contemporary debate focuses on the nature, causes, and importance, or lack of importance, of ethnic differences in intelligence test scores and other measures of cognitive ability, and whether "race" is a meaningful biological construct with significance other than its correlation to membership of particular ethnic groups. Thus, the question of the relative roles of nature and nurture in causing individual and group differences in cognitive ability is seen as fundamental to understanding the debate.[3]

The modern controversy surrounding intelligence and race focuses on the results of IQ studies conducted during the second half of the 20th century in the United States, Western Europe, and other industrialized nations.[4]

Background information

Much of the research on intelligence currently cited is based on IQ testing in the United States. Modern theories and research on race and intelligence are often grounded in two controversial assumptions:

While the g-based factor hierarchy is the most widely accepted current view of the structure of abilities, some theorists regard it as misleading.[5] Moreover, a wide range of human abilities-including many that seem to have intellectual components are outside the domain of standard psychometric tests.[6] Certain environmental factors, such as nutrition, are thought to moderate IQ in children, and other influences have been hypothesized, including education level, richness of the early home environment, the existence of caste-like minorities, socio-economic factors, culture, the effort gap, pidgin language barriers, quality of education, health, racism, lack of positive role-models, exposure to violence, the Flynn effect, sociobiological differences and stereotype threat. One focus of the scientific debate is whether group IQ differences also reflect a genetic component. Hereditarianism hypothesizes that a genetic contribution to intelligence could include genes linked to neuron structure or function, brain size or metabolism, or other physiological differences that could vary with biogeographic ancestry. There is also significant debate about exactly how environmental factors play their role in creating the gap and the interrelationships between these factors. Some researchers focus their attention on intervention techniques to close the gap.

Robert Sternberg writes that race intelligence research that focuses on a genetic cause for the gap is attempting to show that one group is inferior to another group.[7] The conclusions of some researchers: that racial groups in the US vary in average IQ scores, and the hypothesis that a genetic component may be involved, have led to heated academic debates that have spilled over into the public sphere.

Observations about race and intelligence also have important applications for critics of the media portrayal of different races. Stereotypes in media such as books, music, film, and television can reinforce old racist ideas and may influence the perceived opportunities for success in academics for minority students.[8][9]

History

In the 19th and early 20th centuries research on race and intelligence was often used to argue that one race was superior to another, justifying poor outcomes and treatment for the "inferior race".[10] Some early opinions about the differences among races grew out of stereotypes about non-whites developed during the period of colonialism and slavery.[11][12][13][14]

Francisco Gil-White, author of Resurrecting Racism: The Modern Attack on Black People Using Phony Science and Stephen Jay Gould author of The Mismeasure of Man have suggested that some modern research has similar motives.

Slavery and colonialism

Sir Francis Galton wrote on eugenics and psychometrics in the 19th C.
Anthropologist Franz Boas was a prominent 20th C. critic of claims that intelligence differed among races.
Ruth Benedict was an anthropologist who challenged the idea that people of different races had different inherent intelligences.

Because the Atlantic slave trade raised moral questions from its inception, scientific theories about the mental capacities of Black people were provided to justify the enslavement of Africans. According to Alexander Thomas and Samuell Sillen, during this time period the Black man was described as uniquely fitted for bondage because of what researchers at the time called "his primitive psychological organization."[15] Hence, a well-known physician of the antebellum South, Samuel Cartwright of Louisiana, had a psychiatric explanation for runaway slaves. He diagnosed their attempts to gain freedom as a mental illness and coined the term "drapetomania" to describe it.[16]

Scientific arguments about the mental inferiority of Black people were instrumental in keeping slavery alive as an institution in the United States. It was widely regarded that Black people lacked the mental capacity to handle freedom. Secretary of State John C. Calhoun arguing for the extension of slavery in 1844 said, "Here (scientific confirmation) is proof of the necessity of slavery. The African is incapable of self-care and sinks into lunacy under the burden of freedom. It is a mercy to give him the guardianship and protection from mental death."

The writings of Sir Francis Galton, a British psychologist, spurred interest in the study of mental abilities, particularly as they relate to heredity and eugenics.[17] Galton estimated from his field observations in Africa that the African people were "two grades" below Anglo-Saxons' position in the normal frequency distribution of general mental ability. His work was seen as scientific validation of Africans' mental inferiority compared with Anglo-Saxons.[18]

Immigration and segregation

In the 19th and 20th centuries research on race and intelligence has still been used to argue that one race is superior to another, justifying poor outcomes and treatment for the "inferior race".[19] Researchers such as Amanda Thompson and Elazar Barkan have suggested that "Scientific racism" has been used to perpetuate the idea of the intellectual inferiority of African Americans and that it was used to justify segregated education in America.

The scientific debate on the contribution of nature versus nurture to individual and group differences in intelligence can be traced to at least the mid-19th century.[20] Charles Darwin wrote in his Descent of Man (VII, On the races of Man): "Their mental characteristics are likewise very distinct; chiefly as it would appear in their emotional, but partly in their intellectual faculties."

Lewis Terman wrote in The measurement of intelligence in 1916

"(Black and other ethnic minority children) are uneducable beyond the nearest rudiments of training. No amount of school instruction will ever make them intelligent voters or capable citizens in the sense of the world…their dullness seems to be racial, or at least inherent in the family stock from which they come…Children of this group should be segregated in special classes and be given instruction which is concrete and practical. They cannot master abstractions, but they can be made efficient workers…There is no possibility at present of convincing society that they should not be allowed to reproduce, although from a eugenic point of view they constitute a grave problem because of their unusual prolific breeding."

The opinion that there are differences in the brain sizes and brain structures of different racial and ethnic groups was widely held and studied during the 19th century and early 20th century.[21] Average ethnic and racial group differences in IQ were first directly observed when analyzing the data from standardized mental tests administered on large scales during World War I. For example, in this test "Southern Whites", scored below "Northern Negroes."[22] These results inspired the first theories of environmental influences on intelligence. An early advocate of these ideas was Ruth Benedict, who in her book, The Races of Mankind challenged the idea that people of different races had different inherent intelligences.

The difference arose because of differences of income, education, cultural advantages, and other opportunities. --Ruth Benedict

Foremost amongst those researching this was Stanley Porteus, who although not a staff member, gave some lectures at the University of Melbourne, devised his maze test as early as 1913, later applying it in his study of the Aborigines in the Kimberley region and Northern Territory of Australia (1929) and later the Kalahari tribesmen of southern Africa (1934). He also used it to assess the results of pre-frontal brain surgery on mental performance, publishing his results in 1931.[23]

W.O. Brown, writing in The Journal of Negro History in 1931, wrote regarding early intelligence tests:

After the World War and during the severe agitation for the restriction of immigration, aimed especially at the Southeastern Europeans, tests came into a new usage. ..the tests revealed the inferior intelligence of various racial and nationality groups. ..The Southeastern Europeans and the Negroes especially came of badly in these tests. ..The results of the tests elevated their dogma of racial inequality from a mere prejudice to the dignity of a scientifically validated opinion.[24]

Dorthy Roberts writes that the history of the eugenics movement in America was strongly tied to the older scientific racism used to justify slavery. Roberts writes that paralleling the development of eugenic theory was the acceptance of intelligence as the primary indicator of human value. Eugenicists claimed that the IQ test could quantify innate human ability in a single measurement, despite the objections of the creator of the test, Alfred Binet.[25] Beginning in the 1930s, race difference research and hereditarianism — the belief that genetics are the primary cause of differences in intelligence among human groups — began to fall out of favor in psychology and anthropology after major internal debates.[26] In anthropology this occurred in part due to the advocacy of Franz Boas, who in his 1938 edition of The Mind of Primitive Man wrote, "there is nothing at all that could be interpreted as suggesting any material difference in the mental capacity of the bulk of the Negro population as compared with the bulk of the White population."[27] The hereditarian position was challenged by Boas' claim that cranial vault size had increased significantly in the U.S. from one generation to the next, because racial differences in such characteristics had been among the strongest arguments for a genetic role.

Inspired by the American eugenics movement, Nazi Germany implemented the T-4 Euthanasia Program in which roughly 200,000 mentally and physically disabled Germans were killed, and about 400,000 sterilized. The association of hereditarianism with Nazi Germany created a modern academic environment that has been very skeptical of suggestions that there are racial or ethnic differences in measures of intellectual or academic ability and that these differences are primarily determined by genetic factors.[28]

Modern work

File:Charles Murray.gif
Charles Murray (pictured) and Richard Herrnstein started the contemporary debate with The Bell Curve in 1994.

The contemporary scholarly debate on race and intelligence may be traced to Arthur Jensen's 1969 publication in the Harvard Educational Review of "How Much Can We Boost IQ and School Achievement?"[29] In this paper, he wrote on some of the major issues that characterize the genetic hypothesis[30] of racial IQ differences, and on compensatory educational programs. Reports on Jensen's article appeared in Time, Newsweek, Life, U.S. News & World Report, and The New York Times Magazine.

In the 1980s Nobel Prize winner for his work on the development of transistors, William Shockley, postulated that the higher rate of reproduction among US African Americans was having what he termed a "dysgenic" effect (meaning an opposite of eugenics), ; especially as influenced by welfare subsidies (e.g., AFDC), which he opined, unintentionally encouraged childbearing by less productive mothers.[31] He described this work as the most important work of his career, even though it severely tarnished his reputation. Shockley's published writings on this topic, were largely based on the research of Cyril Burt. Shockley also proposed that individuals with IQs below 100 be paid to undergo voluntary sterilization.[32] He was subsequently criticized by the media; however his involvement brought public recognition to several controversial topics.[33]

Press attention returned to the issue of race and intelligence in 1994 with the publication of The Bell Curve, which included two chapters on the subject of racial difference in intelligence and related life outcomes. In response to The Bell Curve, Stephen Jay Gould updated The Mismeasure of Man in 1996.[34] Among other things, he criticized the IQ test as a measure of intelligence, citing what he perceived as inherent racial and social biases as well as systematic flaws in the testing process.

Luigi Luca Cavalli-Sforza has sought to fight racism. On several occasions he publicly debated Arthur Jensen and William Shockley arguing that environmental factors could explain the black-white IQ gap.[35]

Scientific racism

Many studies that purport to be both science-based and attempt to influence public policy have been criticized for scientific racism; the most recent examples of are those of Charles Murray and the late Richard Herrnstein. Melvin Konner, in his book Tangled Wing: Biological Constraints on the Human Spirit Konner accused Murray and Hernstein of trying to make public policy based on speculations about race. He wrote that Rushton's application of a theory drawn from evolutionary biology to the difference between races had no academic legitimacy.[36][37]

In the official statements of position endorsed by the American Sociological Association and the American Anthropological Association,[38] as reported in The New York Times,[39] "A view widespread among many social scientists is that race is not a valid biological concept. However, biologists, particularly the population geneticists who study genetic variation, have found that there is a race structure in the human population; a family tree showing separate branches for Africans, Caucasians (Europe, the Middle East, and the Indian subcontinent), East Asians, Pacific Islanders, and American Indians."[39]

Race

Race as biology

Some geneticists argue race is neither a meaningful concept nor a useful heuristic device,[40] and even that genetic differences among groups are biologically meaningless,[41] on the basis that more genetic variation exists within races than among them,[42] and that racial traits overlap without discrete boundaries.[43] Lewontin, for example argues that there is no biological basis for race on the basis of research indicating that more genetic variation exists within such races than between them. Template:AYref

Some critics of race may not consider this a problem for race and intelligence inquiries. Jared Diamond, who praises Cavalli-Sforza's genetics research over the decades for "demolishing scientists' attempts to classify human populations into races in the same way that they classify birds and other species into races"(Template:AYref), also argues that if such relations exist then "in mental ability New Guineans are probably genetically superior to Westerners" due to that intelligence was likely selected for in hunter-gatherer New Guinea societies where the challenges were tribal warfare and food procurement, compared with high population density European civilizations where the major survival pressure was on genes for resisting epidemics [44] Other geneticists, in contrast, argue that categories of self-identified race/ethnicity or biogeographic ancestry are both valid and useful,[45] that these categories correspond with clusters inferred from multilocus genetic data,[46] and that this correspondence implies that genetic factors might contribute to unexplained phenotypic variation between groups.[47]

A survey taken in 1985, asked 1,200 scientists how many disagree with the following proposition: "There are biological races in the species Homo sapiens." The responses were: biologists 16%, developmental psychologists 36%, physical anthropologists 41%, cultural anthropologists 53%.[48] A survey of cultural and physical anthropologists done in 1999[49] found that the concept of race was rejected by 69% of physical anthropologists and 80% of cultural anthropologists.

Race as a social construct

Robert J. Sternberg, Elena L. Grigorenko, and Kenneth K. Kidd write that the overwhelming portion of the literature on intelligence, race, and genetics is based on folk taxonomies rather than scientific analysis. Race, they write, fits into no known genetic pattern. Race is a socially constructed concept, not a biological one. This concept of race serves a social rather than a biological purpose. Different types of parentage have, at various times and places, given rise to racial labeling (e.g., “Aryan race,” “German race,” and “Jewish race”). Hence race is a highly inconsistent concept. In contemporary North American society, Blacks and coloreds are considered to be one “race,” since any individual who possess any degree of nonwhiteness is automatically grouped in the Black category.[50] (see: One drop rule) In other countries different racial groupings are often employed. In Beyond the Bell Curve: Toward a Model of Talent and Character Development Serge Madhere critiques hereditarian assumptions about ability, biology, and ecology. He argues that the measures of ability assessed on IQ tests are essentially measures of literacy, which is largely a socially constructed outcome. This proposition is validated using data from a large national sample of students and hierarchical regression techniques.[51]

Intelligence

Comparisons of the intelligences of people of different races have often been based on IQ tests. The nature of intelligence and whether or not it can be captured in a single number is a matter of debate.

IQ

All such tests are often called "intelligence tests," though the use of the term "intelligence" is itself controversial. A low but significant correlation was found in tests administered to two groups of kindergarten children in a study reported in 1991[52][53] School grades are the better predicator of later academic success than IQ and the relations may be lower for specific populations. In a sample of 127 students enrolled in a private day school located in a large metropolitan area, the correlations ranged from .11 to .22 with the median of .18.[54]

"Many of the most widely used tests are not intended to measure intelligence itself but some closely related construct: scholastic aptitude, school achievement, specific abilities... . Scores on intelligence-related tests matter, and the stakes can be high," according to the task force appointed by the Board of Scientific Affairs of the American Psychological Association. Such tests are argued to be good measures of the psychometric variable g (for general intelligence factor). While some psychologists regard g as the fundamental measure of intelligence, others emphasize the strengths and weaknesses present in each person's performance on different aspects of the tests.[55]

Although the correlation is fair in some academic areas, the correlation between IQ tests and many real-world results is inconsistent. For example, the hereditary transmission of wealth via IQ is near zero. Some psychologists question the validity of IQ testing and say that aspects of intelligence is not reflected in IQ tests. Criticisms of the validity of IQ testing focuses on questions of test bias. Several conclusions about tests of cognitive ability are now largely accepted by intelligence researchers:[56]

  • IQ scores measure many, but not all of the qualities that people mean by intelligent or smart. (For example, IQ does not measure creativity, wisdom, or personality.)
  • Especially in developing nations, there are many factors that may adversely affect IQ. See Health and intelligence.

Sternberg writes that conventional tests of intelligence can be useful, but only if they are carefully interpreted, taking into account factors such as cross-cultural issues.

Multiple intelligences

Psychologist Howard Gardner says there are multiple forms of intelligence, which he calls multiple intelligences not often captured by the usual IQ tests. Multiple Intelligences can include the following: linguistic; logical-mathematical; spatial; bodily-kinesthetic; musical; naturalistic; interpersonal and intrapersonal. This raises the possibility that it may not be possible to construct a single meaningful ordering on intelligence.

Another theory is the Triarchic theory of intelligence which was formulated by Robert J. Sternberg. According to this theory the three components of intelligence are analytic intelligence, creative intelligence, and practical intelligence. According to Sternberg, only analytic intelligence is measured by standardized IQ tests.

Environment

Steve Sailer has stated that,

"a bad environment can hurt IQ and can be seen in the IQ scores for sub-Saharan African countries. They average only around 70. In contrast, African-Americans average about 85. It appears unlikely that African-Americans’ white admixture can account for most of this 15-point gap because they are only around 17%-18% white on average, according to the latest genetic research. (Thus African-Americans white genes probably couldn't account for more than 3 points of the gap between African-Americans and Africans.) This suggests that the harshness of life in Africa might be cutting ten points or more off African IQ scores."[6]

Research

Test data

The gaps found between the average intelligences of races or ethnicities varies depending on methods used for racial grouping, the method and setting used to test intelligence,[57] the health and economic situation of the test takers, the interplay between the culture of the person taking the test and the culture of those who made the test, and the period in history when the test was performed.

Depending on the way intelligence is measured a variety of gaps may be found between different racial and ethnic groups. Some groups that perform well on one task may do poorly on others. For example, Moroccan and North American individuals were asked in a study by Richard K. Wagner to remember patterns of Oriental rugs and pictures of everyday objects, such as a rooster and a fish. Moroccans, who have long experience in the rug trade, seemed to remember rug patterns better than the North American individuals.[58] Likewise, in 1979 Robert Serpell had Zambian and English children perform a number of tasks. He found that English children did better on a drawing task, but that Zambian children did better on a wire-shaping task.[59]

Attempted world-wide compilations by Herrnstein and Murray, authors of The Bell Curve, Richard Lynn and Rushton of average IQ by race generally place Ashkenazi Jews and East Asians at the top, followed by Whites, Arabs and Native Americans, sub-Saharan Africans and Australian Aboriginals.[60][61] [62]

The IQ scores vary greatly among different nations for the same group. Blacks in Africa score much lower than Blacks in the US. However contrary to indications from the IQ and the Wealth of Nations study, the majority of blacks enrolled in Ivy League Universities in the US are either from Africa or the Caribbean. The chairperson of the sociology department at Harvard University stated: "Since they come from majority-black countries, they are less psychologically handicapped by the stigma of race." This is seen as evidence that racial prejudice combined with the status of being a minority that has been excluded from society does have a significant effect on academic achievement.[63][64] However, according to the African-American economist Thomas Sowell racism and the legacy of slavery do not stand up under scrutiny of historical facts as explanations to the IQ disparity between Blacks and Whites. He argues that "redneck" black culture is the reason for the low IQ and poor academic performance of black Americans.[65] Gaps are seen in other tests of cognitive ability or aptitude, including university admission exams such as the SAT and GRE as well as employment tests for corporate settings and the military (Roth et al. 2001).

Another researcher Philippe Rushton found African university students averaged an IQ of 84. In some studies, by other researchers, they have scored lower (IQ = 77). In still others of our studies, highly-selected engineering students who took math and science courses in high school scored higher (IQ = 103). Rushton also points out that immigrants regardless of race outperform native populations and adds that in theory Africans would revert back to their normal IQ in future generations if kept in the harsh African environment and cultural setting.[66]

Facial recognition ability has also shown differences by race.[67] Richard Ferraro writes that facial recognition is an example of a neuropsychological measure that can be used to assess cognitive abilities that are salient within African-American culture.[68] In the US Blacks' performance is significantly better than that of whites', and blacks are better at recognizing faces of whites than whites are at recognizing blacks.[69] A 1991 study found that white subjects performed significantly more poorly on trials involving African American faces than on trials involving White faces, whereas no such difference was obtained among African American subjects.[70] One possibility is that expertise in perceiving faces of particular races is associated with increased ability to extract information about the spatial relationships between different features[71]. Further research using perceptual tasks could shed light on the specific cognitive processes involved in the other-race effect. [70]

Explanations

Min-Hsiung Huang and Robert M. Hauser found that, controlling for social background, the Black-White test score gap narrowed significantly over the period from 1974 to 1998. For Whites, however, improvement in social background across time does not raise test scores correspondingly.[72]
File:TBC-BW-IQ-SES-withDiff.png
This chart based on data analysis by Herrnstein and Murray, authors of The Bell Curve, was used to present the idea that the test score gap between blacks and whites had not changed when adjusted for economic status. (1994) p. 288[73]
The height of this "ordinary genetically varied corn" is 100% heritable, but the difference between the groups is totally environmental.[74]


The consensus among intelligence researchers is that IQ differences between individuals of the same race reflects functionally and socially significant differences in the intelligence.[75][76][77][78][79][80] There is still substantial debate about the influence of various environmental factors on IQ test score differences between races and ethnic groups in a given country, and whether or not genetics may also play a role.

Test bias

While the existence of average IQ test score differences has been a matter of accepted fact for decades, a great deal of controversy exists among scholars over the question of whether these score differences reflected real differences in cognitive ability. Some claim that there is no evidence for test bias since IQ tests are equally good predictors of IQ-related factors (such as school performance) for U.S. Blacks and Whites.[81] The performance differences persist in tests and testing situations in which care has been taken to eliminate bias.[81] It has also been suggested that IQ tests are formulated in such a way as to disadvantage minorities.[81] Controlled studies have shown that test construction does not substantially contribute to the IQ gap.[81] Still, a 2007 study at Case Western Reserve University found that cultural differences in the provision of information account for racial differences in IQ. The study also found that test problems, similar to some problems found on conventional IQ tests, were only solvable on the basis of specific previous knowledge. Such specific knowledge based questions showed evidence of test bias since the performance on non-specific knowledge based questions did not always correlate with the performance on the knowledge based question.[82] Arguing that IQ tests are often wrongly described as measuring "innate" rather than developed ability, Template:AYref write that this "labeling bias" causes people to inappropriately attribute the Black-White gap to "innate" differences.[83] They argue that non-cultural environmental factors cause gaps measured by the tests, rather than innate difference based on genetics, and that to use these tests as a measure of innate difference is misleading and improper.[84]

Increases in IQ scores over time

William T. Dickens and James R. Flynn write that blacks have gained 5 or 6 IQ points on non-Hispanic whites between 1972 and 2002. This graph shows the gains for various tests.[85]

The secular, international increase in test scores, commonly called the Flynn effect, is seen by Flynn and others as reason to expect the eventual convergence of average black and white IQ scores. Flynn argues that the average IQ scores in several countries have increased about 3 points per decade during the 20th century, which he and others attribute predominantly to environmental causes.[86] This means, given the same test, the mean black American performance today could be higher than the mean white American performance in 1920, though the gains causing this appear to have occurred predominantly in the lower half of the IQ distribution.[87] If changes in environment can cause changes in IQ over time, they argue, then contemporary differences between groups could also be due to an unknown environmental factor. On the supposition that the effect started earlier for whites, because their social and economical conditions began to improve earlier than did those of blacks, they anticipate that the IQ gap among races might change in the future or is even now changing. An added complication to this hypothesis is the question of whether the secular IQ gains can be predominantly a real change in cognitive ability. Flynn's face-value answer to this question is "No",[88] and other researchers have found reason to concur. Template:AYref wrote that "the gains cannot be explained solely by increases at the level of the latent variables (common factors), which IQ tests purport to measure".

Racism and discrimination

Researchers such as Jack Demaine find racial categorizations problematic in educational settings.[89] Racial categorizations, Jack Demaine writes, may have adverse impacts on the education of minorities. Similarly, Alastair Bonnett, Bruce Carrington state:

The collection of ethnic and racial statistics has become common in a growing number of institutional settings. Yet contemporary approaches to race and ethnicity suggest that the very process of compelling people to assign themselves to one of a small number of racial or ethnic 'boxes' is, at best, essentialist and, at worst, racist.[90]

Stereotype threat
An experiment on college students in 1995 showed the impact of Stereotype threat by asking students to fill out a form before taking the test indicating their race. The scores in this graph have been adjusted by SAT.[91]

Stereotype threat is the fear that one's behavior will confirm an existing stereotype of a group with which one identifies. This fear may in turn lead to an impairment of performance (Aronson, Wilson, & Akert, 2005). Stereotype threat has been documented by the social psychologists Claude Steele, Joshua Aronson, Irwin Katz, and Steven Spencer, who have conducted several studies on this topic.

"When capable black college students fail to perform as well as their white counterparts, the explanation often has less to do with preparation or ability than with the threat of stereotypes about their capacity to succeed."
- Claude M. Steele, The Atlantic Monthly, August 1999
Thin Ice: Stereotype Threat and Black College Students

Steele and Aronson write that making race salient when taking a test of cognitive ability negatively affected high-ability African American students.[92] Steele writes that the stigma of being African American is still relevant, as it has an effect on the educational outcomes of African Americans. Stereotypes such as: Asian-Americans excelling in mathematics or African-Americans always testing poorly can be extremely harmful. Stereotype threats can seriously alter academic achievement and motivation.[93]

In a paper prepared for an APA convention, Steele writes: "Thus the predicament of 'stereotype vulnerability': The group members then know that anything about them or anything they do that fits the stereotype can be taken as confirming it as self-characteristic, in the eyes of others, and perhaps even in their own eyes. This vulnerability amounts to a jeopardy of double devaluation: once for whatever bad thing the stereotype-fitting behavior or feature would say about anyone, and again for its confirmation of the bad things alleged in the stereotype."

Steele and Aronson are not first to test stereotype threat. During the 1960s Irwin Katz, psychologist, suggested that stereotype threat could also influence performance on IQ tests. Katz found that Blacks were able to score better an IQ subtest if the test was presented as a test of eye-hand coordination. Blacks also scored higher on an IQ test when they believe the test will be compared to that of other blacks.[94] Katz concluded that his subjects were thoroughly aware of the judgment of intellectual inferiority held by many white Americans. With little expectation of overruling this judgment, their motivation was low, and so were their scores.[95] Paul Sackett, a psychologist agrees that stereotype threat is a real phenomenon and that it is is a potentially important contributor to the racial achievement gap. He cautions however, that these findings may be widely misinterpreted to mean that eliminating stereotype threat eliminates the entire Black-White performance gap, and encourages researchers to continue their study of this and other phenomena. [96]

Physiological responses to racism

Stereotype threat can result in physiological responses that can be measured objectively. For example, a study by Blascovich J, Spencer SJ, Quinn D and Steele C. reported that African Americans under stereotype threat exhibited larger increases in arterial blood pressure during an academic test, and performed more poorly on difficult test items. Some researchers feel this may explain the higher death rates from hypertension related disorders among African Americans.[97] A study by Toni Schmader and Michael Johns reported that stereotype threat can effectively reduce working memory capacity, another factor in poor test performance.[98] Stereotype threat may undermine intellectual performance by triggering a disruptive mental load. Jean-Claude Croizet, Gérard Després, Marie-Eve Gauzins, Pascal Huguet, Jacques-Philippe Leyens and Alain Méot reported increased heart rates for test subjects operating under stereotype threat.[99]

Quality of education

Some researches have written that studies that find test performance gaps between races even after adjusting for education level, such as the analysis found in The Bell Curve, fail to adjust for the quality of education. Not all high school graduates or college graduates have received the same quality of education. A 2006 study reported that that years of education is an inadequate measure of the educational experience among multicultural elders, and that adjusting for quality of education greatly reduced the overall effect of racial differences on the tests.[100] A 2004 study reported that quality of education and cultural experience influence how older African Americans approach neuropsychological tasks and concluded that adjustment for these variables may improve specificity of neuropsychological measures.[101] Yet another study reported that, although significant differences were observed between the ethnic groups when matched for years of education, equating for literacy level eliminated all performance differences between African Americans and Whites on both cancellation tasks which assess visual scanning.[102](Like reaction time tests cancellation task tests are sometimes regarded as "culture free" tests of intelligence.) Eric A. Hanushek and Steven G. Rivkin wrote in their 2006 book that unequal distributions of inexperienced teachers and of racial concentrations in schools can explain all of the increased achievement gap between grades 3 and 8.[103]

A 2004 study in South Africa found highly significant effects for both level and quality of education within the black African first language groups taking the Wechsler IQ tests. Scores black African first language groups with advantaged education were comparable with the US standardization, whereas scores for black African first language participants with disadvantaged education were significantly lower than this. The study cautioned that faulty conclusions may be drawn about the effects of ethnicity and the potential for neuropsychological misdiagnosis.[104]

Racial discrimination in education

Roslyn Arlin Mickelson writes that racial discrimination in education arises from actions of institutions or individual state actors, their attitudes and ideologies, or processes that systematically treat students from different racial/ethnic groups disparately or inequitably.[105] Despite advancement in education reform efforts, to this day African American students continue to experience inequities within the educational system. Hala Elhoweris , Kagendo Mutua, Negmeldin Alsheikh and Pauline Holloway conducted a study of the effect of students' ethnicity on teachers' educational decision making. The results of this study indicated that the student's ethnicity did make a difference in the teachers' referral decisions for gifted and talented educational programs.[106]Recently, a number of scholars have examined the issue of disproportionate representation of minority students in special education programs [107][108]

Teachers' perceptions of a students cultural background may effect school achievement. African American students with African American cultural backgrounds, for example, have been found to benefit from culturally responsive teaching.[109] In a 2003 study researchers found that teachers perceived students with African American culture-related movement styles as lower in achievement, higher in aggression, and more likely to need special education services than students with standard movement styles irrespective of race or other academic indicators. [110]

Ellis Cose writes that low expectations may have a negative impact on the achievement of minorities. He writes that black people did not need to read The Bell Curve to be aware of the low expectations held for them by the majority culture. He recalls examples of low expectations from his teachers in school who regarded his use of AAVE as "laziness" and teachers who did not feel it was important to purchase new text books because they did not expect the students to be able to read anything complex. He contrasts these low expectations with the high expectations philosophy of Xavier University where, using the ideas Whimbey articulated in his book Intelligence can be Taught teachers created a program called SOAR. SOAR raised the performance of black students and lead Xavier to become the university that sends the greatest number of black students to medical school in the United States. The SOAR program produced gains equivalent to 120 points on an SAT test. Cose writes that "..we must treat people, whatever their color, as if they have unlimited intellectual capacity."[111]

Caste

This table illustrates how social status or caste position is related to test scores and school success in nations around the world. Source: Inequality by Design: Cracking the Bell Curve Myth by Claude S. Fischer, Michael Hout, Martín Sánchez Jankowski, Samuel R. Lucas, Ann Swidler, and Kim Vos[112]

Group Differences Around the World
  Status or Caste Position Test Scores, School Success
Country High Low High Low
United States[113] Whites Blacks Whites Blacks
  Whites Latinos Whites Latinos
  Whites American Indians[114] Whites American Indians
Great Britain[115] Great Britain Irish, Scottish English Irish, Scottish
Northern Ireland[116] Protestants Catholics Protestants Catholics
Australia[117] Whites Aborigines Whites Aborigines
New Zealand[118] Whites Maoris Whites Maoris
South Africa[119] English Afrikaaners English Afrikaaners
Belgium[120] French Flemish French Flemish
Israel[121] Jews Arabs Jews Arabs
  Western Jews Eastern Jews Western Jews Eastern Jews
India[122] Nontribals Tribal people Nontribals Tribal people
  Brahmin Harijan Brahmin Harijan
  High caste Low caste High caste Low caste
Czechoslovakia[123] Slovaks Gypsies Slovaks Gypsies
Japan[124] Non-Burakumin Burakumin Non-Burakumin Burakumin
  Japanese Origin Korean Origin Japanese Origin Korean Origin
South Korea[125] Koreans Southeast Asians Koreans Southeast Asians

These results, just like the inferior test scores of Eastern and Southern Europeans immigrants in the United States 75 years ago, may represent a social division that leads to the gaps in test scores, rather than a pre-established and "natural" hierarchy of "races." In other words, these divisions, are closely aligned with local "social constructs" of race, the outcomes for ethnic groups are, in the opinion of these authors, a result of the social structure rather than confirmation of its validity.[112]

Health

Regarding the IQ gaps in the U.S., numerous explanations beside genetics have been proposed. Joel Wiesen lists more than a hundred.[126] Increased rates of low birth weight babies and lower rates of breastfeeding in Blacks as compared to Whites are some factors of many that have been proposed to affect the IQ gap. The Flynn effect is often cited as evidence that average IQ scores have changed greatly and rapidly, for reasons poorly understood, noting that average IQ in the US may have been below 75 before the start of this effect, and thus some argue that the IQ gap between races could change in the future or is changing, especially if the effect started earlier for Whites.

Genetics

See also The genome and intelligence

A few of the notable proponents of the partly genetic hypothesis are Raymond B. Cattell, Arthur Jensen and Hans Eysenck.

Rushton and Jensen examined 10 categories of research evidence from around the world to contrast "a hereditarian model" (50% genetic-50% cultural) and a culture-only model (0% genetic-100% cultural). In the article "Thirty Years of Research on Race Differences in Cognitive Ability" published in the APA journal Psychology, Public Policy and Law they cite the following evidence to support the hereditarian model:[127][128]

Some scholars have proposed that in order to make a racial hypothesis about intelligence the genes for intelligence need to be identified along with their frequencies in the various populations. However recent studies attempting to find regions in the genome relating to intelligence have had little success. A recent study used several hundred people in two groups, one with a very high IQ, average 160, and a control group with an average IQ of 102. By the fifth step the study could not find a single gene that was related to intelligence. Critics of these studies say the failure to find a specific gene associated with intelligence is indicative of the complex nature of intelligence. They contend that intelligence is probably under the influence of several genes. Some estimate that as much as 40% of the genome may contribute to intelligence.[129]

Recently scientists at the University of Chicago identified two genes, microcephalin and ASPM. Mutations in these genes are associated with brain size abnormality, microcephaly. The normal variants are found at high frequencies in Asian and European populations but they are not found among Sub-Saharan Africans. The scientists stated that microcephalin may have arisen some 37,000 years ago coinciding with upper paleolithic transitions in Europe. They also stated that a variant of APSM arose about 5,800 years ago roughly correlating with the development of written language, spread of agriculture and development of cities. They thus believe these two genes conferred some cognitive abilities upon Asians and Europeans. [130]

Other scholars have criticized the University of Chicago scientists because they made claims about these genes without undertaking any direct experimentation to test their hypothesis on increased intelligence and brain size. Subsequently when these experiments were done, no relationship was found between these genes and intelligence or brain size. [131][132] Critics of these studies also say that as long as social and environmental disparities between the races exist it will be impossible to scientifically test whether there are any genetic differences in IQ between the various populations. They propose that if the historical effects of poverty and social bigotry were eliminated and differences in IQ between the races still persisted then there might be some utility in such research.[133]

Theories on the intelligence of Ashkenazi Jews

A link between disease mutations specific to the Ashkenazi Jews and high IQ scores has been suggested by scientists at the University of Utah, who cite evidence that sphingolipid disorders promote the growth and interconnection of brain cells and that mutations in the DNA repair genes, involved in some Ashkenazic diseases, may also unleash growth of neurons. The researchers predict that these disease mutations will enhance IQ in heterozygotes. This prediction is based on evidence that selection pressure has increased the frequency of the disease mutations in the reproductively isolated Ashkenazi population in medieval times. The hypothesis has not yet been empirically tested.[134]

Ashkenazi Jews have been reported to score 0.75 to 1.0 standard deviations above the general European average, corresponding to an IQ 112-115.[135][136][137]

According to Richard Lynn the most accurate reading of the IQ of Jews in Britain is 110. Lynn proposes that the overrepresentation of Jews as Nobel Prize Winners and as Fellows of the Royal Society can be partly explained by the higher average Jewish IQ because comparatively small differences in average intelligence can become very large differences in the very high I.Q ranges. Notable Jewish intellectuals include Einstein, Freud and Marx. Jews are over-represented among Nobel prize-winners by a factor of 8.0 in Britain and 12.3 in the United States.[138][139]

Interpretations

Given the observed differences in IQ scores between certain groups, a great deal of debate revolves around the significance of these observations. Various interpretations of test data lead to a multitude of conflicting conclusions as to which specific explanations the data support.

Some people have attributed differential economic growth between nations to differences in the intelligence of their populations. One example is Richard Lynn's IQ and the Wealth of Nations. The book is sharply criticized in the peer-reviewed paper The Impact of National IQ on Income and Growth.[140] Another peer-reviewed paper, Intelligence, Human Capital, and Economic Growth: An Extreme-Bounds Analysis, finds a strong connection between intelligence and economic growth.[141] It has been argued that East Asian nations underachieve compared to IQ scores.

Jared Diamond's Guns, Germs and Steel instead argues that historical differences in economic and technological development for different areas can be explained by differences in geography (which affects factors like population density and spread of new technology) and differences in available crops and domesticatable animals.[142] However,psychologist John Philippe Rushton suggests these environmental differences may operate in part by selecting for higher levels of IQ[143] There is no evidence to suggest that such selective forces occur in regards to IQ[citation needed]. Consensus at the American Psychological Association is that a partly genetic hypothesis is as of now, inadequate in explaining differences in IQ among population groups.[144]


Prejudice and IQ

A university study in the US was carried out in order to determine whether a relationship existed between prejudice and IQ. Students were given an IQ test and a test that measures racial prejudice. The study found that students who scored lower on IQ tests were more prejudiced. [145]

Media portrayal

Media portrayal of race and intelligence in various mediums, such as films, books, and newspapers, characterize people of various races to be more or less intelligent. Likewise, reporting on research into race and intelligence has been criticized: either for giving scientific theories of race too much credit, or for rejecting the theories of some researchers in the name of racial harmony.

Examples

Critics of contemporary media have highlighted portrayals of minorities as less intelligent[146] (or in the case of Asians, on occasion more intelligent[147]) in films and movies.

Black intelligence stereotypes

Early stereotypes
Early minstrel shows lampooned the supposed stupidity of Blacks. Detail from cover of The Celebrated Negro Melodies, as Sung by the Virginia Minstrels, 1843

Early minstrel shows lampooned the supposed stupidity of Blacks, movies such as Birth of a Nation questioned whether or not Black people were fit to run for governmental offices or vote. Secretary of State John C. Calhoun arguing for the extension of slavery in 1844 said "Here (scientific confirmation) is proof of the necessity of slavery. The African is incapable of self-care and sinks into lunacy under the burden of freedom. It is a mercy to give him the guardianship and protection from mental death."

Even after slavery ended the intellectual capacity of Black people was still frequently questioned. Lewis Terman wrote in The measurement of intelligence in 1916

"(Black and other ethnic minority children) are uneducable beyond the nearest rudiments of training. No amount of school instruction will ever make them intelligent voters or capable citizens in the sense of the world…their dullness seems to be racial, or at least inherent in the family stock from which they come…Children of this group should be segregated in special classes and be given instruction which is concrete and practical. They cannot master abstractions, but they can be made efficient workers…There is no possibility at present of convincing society that they should not be allowed to reproduce, although from a eugenic point of view they constitute a grave problem because of their unusual prolific breeding."

Modern stereotypes
Some regard Jar Jar as a thinly veiled version of the type of portrayals used in minstrelsy to lampoon the supposed stupidity of Black people.

Patricia J. Williams, writer for The Nation, said this of Jar Jar Binks, a character from the 2002 Star Wars film: "...intentionally or not, Jar Jar's pratfalls and high jinks borrow heavily from the genre of minstrelsy. Despite the amphibian get-up, his relentless, panicky, manchild-like idiocy is imported directly from the days of Amos 'N' Andy." Many aspects of Jar Jar's character are believed to be highly reminiscent of the archetypes portrayed in blackface minstrelsy.[148]

According to Robert M. Entman an Andrew Rojecki, authors of the The Black Image in the White Mind, in television and film Black characters are less likely to be the "the intellectual drivers of its problem solving." Entman and Rojeki assert that media images of Blacks may have profound effects on the perceptions by both Blacks and Whites about black intellectual potential.[149]

Contemporary sports commentators have questioned whether blacks are intelligent enough to hold "strategic" positions or coach games such as football.[150] In another example, a study of the portrayal of race, ethnicity and nationality in televised sporting events by journalist Derrick Jackson in 1989 showed that blacks were more likely than Whites to be described in demeaning intellectual terms.[151] Political activist and one-time presidential candidate Rev. Jesse Jackson said in 1985 that the news media portray blacks as less intelligent than we are.[152] Film director Spike Lee explains that these images have negative impacts. "In my neighborhood, we looked up to athletes, guys who got the ladies, and intelligent people," said Lee. "[Now] If you're intelligent, you're called a white guy or girl."[153]

Even so-called positive images of Black people can lead to stereotypes about intelligence. In Darwin's Athletes: how sport has damaged Black America and preserved the myth of race, John Hoberman writes that the prominence of African-American athletes encourages a de-emphasis on academic achievement in black communities.[154] In a 1997 study on racial stereotypes in sports, participants were shown a photograph of a white or a black basketball player. They then listened to a recorded radio broadcast of a basketball game. White photographs were rated as exhibiting significantly more intelligence in the way they played the game, even though the radio broadcast and target player represented by the photograph were the same throughout the trial.[155] Several other authors have said that sports coverage that highlights 'natural black athleticism' has the effect of suggesting white superiority in other areas, such as intelligence.[156]

East Asian intelligence stereotypes

File:The Doctor.jpg
Fu Manchu.

Asians have generally been portrayed in the media as intelligent but unsociable.[157] The early 20th century fictional character Fu Manchu was one startling example of this kind of media portrayal:

Imagine a person, tall, lean and feline, high-shouldered, with a brow like Shakespeare and a face like Satan, a close-shaven skull, and long, magnetic eyes of the true cat-green. Invest him with all the cruel cunning of an entire Eastern race, accumulated in one giant intellect, with all the resources of science past and present... Imagine that awful being, and you have a mental picture of Dr. Fu-Manchu, the yellow peril incarnate in one man. –The Insidious Dr. Fu Manchu

White intelligence stereotypes

File:Irish-stereotypes.jpg
The cartoon above (New Physiognomy, New York, 1866), contrasts Florence Nightingale, the Civil War nurse, with "Bridget McBruiser", the stereotypical Irish woman.
Scientific Racism from an American magazine, Harper’s Weekly, says that the Irish are similar to 'Negroes' and wonders why both groups are not extinct.

The social definition of "White" has changed over the years, and several White groups have at times been portrayed by the media as unintelligent. This includes ethnic groups such as the British, Irish, and Slavs.[158]

English intelligence stereotypes

The English people are stereotyped as inordinately proper, prudish, and stiff and as having bad teeth.[159] Characters in historical movies often have English accents even when the setting has nothing to do with England. Upper-class characters are also often given English accents. In more recent times, many movie villains, including Jafar from Aladdin, Scar from The Lion King, Hans Gruber from Die Hard, and Hannibal Lecter from The Silence of the Lambs, have all been portrayed by British actors or given English accents.

Notably, in Disney films from the 1990s onward, English accents are generally employed to serve one of two purposes: slapstick comedy or evil genius.[160] Examples include Aladdin (the Sultan and Jafar, respectively), The Lion King (Zazu and Scar, respectively), The Hunchback of Notre Dame (Victor the Gargoyle and Frollo, respectively), and Pocahontas (Wiggins and Ratcliffe, respectively, both of whom happen to be played by the same actor, American David Ogden Stiers).

These two stereotypes are compounded in a scene in Pocahontas, in which Ratcliffe menacingly mentions giving the savages a "proper English greeting", in response to which Wiggins holds up two gift baskets.

Irish intelligence stereotypes

Although the Irish, Germans, French, etc are considered ethnic groups today, the common term in the 19th century was "race". Much was made of Celtic versus Anglo-Saxon racial characteristics, regarding historic identity and behavior patterns. An analysis of nineteenth-century British attitudes by Mary J. Hickman and Bronwen Walter wrote that the 'Irish Catholic' was one viewed as an "other," or a different race in the construction of the British nationalist myth. Likewise the Irish considered the English "other" and fought hard to break away and create their own homeland, which they finally did in the 1920s. [161]

One 19th century British cartoonist even depicted Irish immigrants as ape-like and as racially different. One American doctor in the 1850s James Redfield, argued that "facial angle" was a sign of intelligence and character. He likened the facial characteristics of the human races to animals. Thus Irishmen resembled dogs, Yankees were like bears, Germans like lions, Negroes like elephants, Englishmen like bulls, Turks like turkeys, Persians like peacocks, Greeks like sheep, Hindus like swans, Jews like goats, and Frenchmen like frogs. [7] In the 20th century physical stereotypes survived in the comic books until the 1950s, with Irish characters like Mutt and Jeff, and Jiggs and Maggie appearing daily in hundreds of newspapers. [162]

Jewish intelligence stereotypes

Modern European antisemitism has its origin in 19th century theories—now mostly considered as pseudo-scientific—that said that the Semitic peoples, including the Jews, are entirely different from the Aryan, or Indo-European, populations, and that they can never be amalgamated with them. In this view, Jews are not opposed on account of their religion, but on account of their supposed hereditary or genetic racial characteristics including: greed, a special aptitude for money-making and low cunning.

In early films such as Cohen's Advertising Scheme (1904, silent) stereotyped Jews as "scheming merchants"[163]

To this day Jewish people are sometimes stereotyped in media as being intellectually gifted.[164]

Portrayal of research by the media

The Bell Curve

File:Charles Murray.gif
Charles Murray

The Bell Curve is a controversial, best-selling 1994 book by Richard J. Herrnstein and Charles Murray exploring the role of intelligence in American life. The book became widely read and debated due to its discussion of race and intelligence in Chapters 13 and 14.

Press coverage has given considerable positive attention to theories of genetic racial differences in intelligence even though there is no consensus among researchers regarding their validity.[165] Upon publication, The Bell Curve received a great deal of positive publicity, including cover stories in Newsweek ("the science behind [it] is overwhelmingly mainstream"), early publication (under protest by other writers and editors) by The New Republic by its editor-in-chief at the time Andrew Sullivan, and The New York Times Book Review (which suggested critics disliked its "appeal to sweet reason" and are "inclined to hang the defendants without a trial"). Early articles and editorials appeared in Time, The New York Times ("makes a strong case"), The New York Times Magazine, Forbes, the Wall Street Journal, and National Review. It received a respectful airing on such shows as Nightline, the MacNeil/Lehrer NewsHour, the McLaughlin Group, Think Tank, PrimeTime Live, and All Things Considered. [8]

The positive reception of The Bell Curve in media such as newspapers and television talk shows was troubling to critics such as economist Edward S. Herman and evolutionary biologist Joseph L. Graves who felt that it indicated a troubling acceptance of what Herman calls deterministic racist doctrines.[166] Dennis M. Rutledge suggests that through soundbites of works like Jensen's famous study on the achievement gap, and Herrnstein and Murray's book The Bell Curve, the media "paints a picture of Blacks and other people of color as collective biological illiterates — as not only intellectually unfit but evil and criminal as well," thus providing, he says "the logic and justification for those who would further disenfranchise and exclude racial and ethnic minorities."[167]

APA response

In response to the controversy surrounding The Bell Curve, the American Psychological Association's Board of Scientific Affairs in 1995 established a special task force to publish an investigative report on the research presented in the book. Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns. Regarding genetic causes, they judged that there is not much direct evidence on this point, but what little there is fails to support the genetic hypothesis. The January 1997 issue of American Psychologist included eleven critical responses to the APA report, most of which criticized the report's failure to examine all of the evidence for or against the partly-genetic interpretation of racial differences in IQ.[citation needed]

Stereotype threat

Stereotype threat is the fear that one's behavior will confirm an existing stereotype of a group with which one identifies. This fear may in turn lead to an impairment of performance (Aronson, Wilson, & Akert, 2005). Stereotype threat was first articulated and documented by the social psychologists Claude Steele, Joshua Aronson, and Steven Spencer, who have conducted several studies on this topic.

While Stereotype threat has not received as much media attention as The Bell Curve much of the media coverage has been positive. The Atlantic Monthly ran a feature article on the topic authored by psychologist Claude M. Steele in August 1999.[168] Still one conservative researcher feels that the coverage has been inaccurate. In a 2004 study Sackett said he found indications of widespread and systematic research misinterpretation regarding one of the more popular explanations for the IQ gap.[169] Introducing stereotype threat to a test-taking environment has been shown to increase the existing gap between Blacks or Whites in relation to Whites or Asians respectively, and has thus been offered as a potential contributor to the gap.[170] However Sackett said 88% of accounts in the popular media, 91% in scientific journals, and 67% in psychology textbooks had misinterpreted the findings as that eliminating the introduced stereotype threat eliminated the Black-White gap, when in fact the students had already been matched according to prior scores.[171] Sackett suggests the appeal of the misinterpreted findings may have been a factor, and that such research results in general may in this way be systemically more readily accepted.[172]

Snyderman and Rothman

The Snyderman and Rothman study accused the media of liberal bias in reporting on race and intelligence. Mark Snyderman and Stanley Rothman argued in their joint paper in 1988 that media coverage of intelligence-related research is often inaccurate and misleading. They surveyed the opinions of journalists and science editors and intelligence experts (not necessarily with knowledge about race), including scholars in the subfields of psychology, sociology, cognitive science, education, and genetics. They argue that media coverage of intelligence related topics was overall inaccurate and misleading. They say the media has misreported the views of the scientific community, especially about the role of genetic and environmental factors in explaining individual and group differences in IQ.

In their 1987 survey, they wrote:

Forty-five percent believe the difference to be a product of both genetic and environmental variation, compared to only 15% who feel the difference is entirely due to environmental variation. Twenty-four percent of experts do not believe there are sufficient data to support any reasonable opinion, and 14% did not respond to the question. Eight experts (1%) indicate a belief in an entirely genetic determination.

No poll option was provided to indicate "predominantly (but not entirely) environmental.

They found that the media regularly presented the views of Stephen Jay Gould and Leon Kamin as representative of mainstream opinion among experts, whereas those who stress that individual and group differences may be substantially genetic (e.g., Arthur Jensen) are characterized as a minority. According to Synderman and Rothman, their survey of expert opinion found that the opposite is true, however proportion of experts supporting these hypotheses today is unknown.

Surveys of academic opinion

A survey was conducted in 1987 of a broad sample of 1,020 scholars in specialties that would give them reason to be knowledgeable about IQ (but not necessarily about race). The survey was given to members of the American Education Research Association, National Council on Measurement in Education, American Psychological Association, American Sociological Association, Behavior Genetics Association, and Cognitive Science Society. According to the report, regarding the question "The source of black-white difference in IQ":

This is perhaps the central question in the IQ controversy. Respondents were asked to express their opinion of the role of genetic differences in the black-white IQ differential. Forty-five percent believe the difference to be a product of both genetic and environmental variation, compared to only 15% who feel the difference is entirely due to environmental variation. Twenty-four percent of experts do not believe there are sufficient data to support any reasonable opinion, and 14% did not respond to the question. Eight experts (1%) indicate a belief in an entirely genetic determination.[173]

Robert Sternberg cautioned against supposing that the survey represented anything but opinion saying, "science isn't done by majority rule".[174] Respondents on average called themselves slightly left of center politically, but political and social opinions accounted for less than 10% of the variation in responses. Carol Swain, author of The New White Nationalism reacted with some dismay to the survey, stating:

At least one important survey suggests that a belief in the biological inferiority of some races in regard to intelligence is more common than generally supposed. Smith College professor Stanley Rothman and Harvard researcher Mark Snyderman surveyed a sample of mostly scientific experts in the field of educational psychology in the late 1980s and found that 53 percent believed IQ differences between whites and African Americans were at least partly genetic in origin, while only 17 percent attributed the IQ differences to environmental factors alone (the remainder either believed the data was currently insufficient to decide the issue or refused to answer the question).

According to the American Psychological Association's 1995 task force report on intelligence research:

It is sometimes suggested that the Black/White differential in psychometric intelligence is partly due to genetic differences (Jensen, 1972). There is not much direct evidence on this point, but what little there is fails to support the genetic hypothesis.[175]

The APA subsequently published eleven critical responses in 1997, most arguing that the report failed to examine adequately the evidence for the genetic hypothesis.[30][176] Charles Murray, for instance, responded:

Actually, there is no direct evidence at all, just a wide variety of indirect evidence, almost all of which the task force chose to ignore.[177]

The report did agree with many of the non-race-based statements on intelligence made in The Bell Curve[178] and concludes with a call for more reflection in debates on intelligence and for a "shared and sustained effort" in more research to answer the many unanswered questions that remain.[179] Coming advances in genetics and genomics are expected to soon provide the ability to test hypotheses about group differences more rigorously than has as yet been possible.[180]

Researchers who believe that there is no significant genetic contribution to race differences in intelligence include Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, and Template:AYref. Some scientists who emphasize cultural explanations do not necessarily exclude a small genetic influence. Template:A(Y)ref suggests up to 20% genetic influence be included in the cultural explanation. Researchers who believe that there are significant genetic contributions to race differences in intelligence include Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, and Template:AYref.

Opinions of scholars and others

A survey was conducted in 1987 of a broad sample of 1,020 scholars (65% replied) in specialties that would give them reason to be knowledgeable about IQ (but not necessarily about race; Snyderman & Rothman, 1987). The survey was given to members of the American Education Research Association, National Council on Measurement in Education, American Psychological Association, American Sociological Association, Behavior Genetics Association, and Cognitive Science Society. Political and social opinions, reported in the same survey, accounted for less than 10% of the variation in responses. (Respondents on average called themselves slightly left of center politically.) Measures of expertise or eminence accounted for little or no variation in responses.

One question was "Which of the following best characterizes your opinion of the heritability of the Black-White difference in I.Q.?" (emphasis original).[181] The responses were divided into five categories:

  • The difference is entirely due to environmental variation: 15%.
  • The difference is entirely due to genetic variation: 1% (8 respondents).
  • The difference is a product of both genetic and environmental variation: 45%.
  • The data are insufficient to support any reasonable opinion: 24%.
  • No response (or not qualified): 14%.
A selection of survey results
Question Responses
What heritability would you estimate for IQ differences within the White population? Average estimate of 60 (± 17) percent.
What heritability would you estimate for IQ differences within the Black population? Average estimate of 57 (± 18) percent.
Are intelligence tests biased against Blacks? On a scale of 1 (not at all or insignificantly) to 4 (extremely), mean response of 2 (somewhat).
What is the source of the average Black-White difference in IQ? Both genetic and environmental (45%, or 52% of those responding).

The age of the survey and the anonymity of the respondents could constrain its interpretation.

In a 1988 survey, journalists, editors, and IQ experts were asked their "opinion of the source of the black-white difference in IQ" Template:AYref

Group Entirely Environment Entirely Genetic Both Data Are Insufficient
Journalists 34% 1% 27% 38%
Editors 47% 2% 23% 28%
IQ Experts 17% 1% 53% 28%
The view of the American Psychological Association

In response to the controversy surrounding The Bell Curve, the American Psychological Association's Board of Scientific Affairs in 1995 established a special task force to publish an investigative report on the research presented in the book.[182]

The task force agrees that there do exist large differences between the average IQ scores of blacks and whites, and that these differences cannot be attributed to biases in test construction, nor does it "simply reflect differences in socio-economic status". While they admit there is no empirical evidence supporting it, the APA task force suggests that explanations based on social status and cultural differences may be possible. Regarding genetic causes, they noted, "There is not much direct evidence on this point, but what little there is fails to support the genetic hypothesis." The January 1997 issue of American Psychologist included eleven critical responses to the APA report, most of which criticized the report's failure to examine all of the evidence for or against the genetic hypothesis of racial differences in IQ.

Controversies

Utility of research

Theories of race and intelligence have been challenged on grounds of their utility. Critics want to know what purpose such research could serve and why it has been an intense an area of focus for a few researchers. Some defend the research, saying it has egalitarian aims or that it is pure science, others say that the true motivation for the research is the same as that of the eugenics movement and other forms of scientific racism.[183][184] Even supporters of intelligence research have described such research as analogous to "working with dynamite" or "dangerous play" in sports.[185]

As to whether research in this area is desirable, John C. Loehlin wrote in 1992, "Research on racial differences in intelligence is desirable if the research is appropriately motivated, honestly done, and adequately communicated." [emphasis original] Defenders of the research suggest that both scientific curiosity and a desire to draw benefits from the research are appropriate motivations. Researchers such as Richard Lynn have suggested that conclusions from the research can help make political decisions, such as the type of educational opportunities and expectations of achievement policy makers should have for people of different races. Charles Murray, a conservative political pundit at the American Enterprise Institute has used their conclusions to criticize social programs based on racial equality that fail, he claims, to recognize the realities of racial differences.

In a book review J. Philippe Rushton called Richard Lynn's and Tatu Vanhanen's book IQ And Global Inequality which links GDP with the racial composition of nations as "the most important contribution to economic understanding since Adam Smith".[186]

Sociologist and demographer Reanne Frank says that some race and intelligence research has been abused "The most malignant are the "true believers," who subscribe to the typological distinctions that imply hierarchical rankings of worth across different races. Although this group remains small, the members' work is often widely publicized and well known (e.g., Herrnstein and Murray 1994; Rushton 1991)"[187]

Potential for bias

Proponents of genetic explanations of race/IQ correlation have often been criticized both of scientific misconduct and of their intimate links with groups that have historic ties to Nazis and eugenics of the early 19th century, such as the Pioneer Fund. The Pioneer Fund has been characterized by the Southern Poverty Law Center as a hate group. Beverly Daniel Tatum writes that dominant cultures often set the parameters by which minority cultures will be judged. Minority groups are labeled as substandard in significant ways, for example blacks have historically been characterized as less intelligent than whites. Tatum suggests that the ability to set these parameters is a form of white privilege.[188] Proponents of genetic explanations of race/IQ correlation have in turn accused their critics of suppressing scientific debate in the name of political correctness. They claim harassment and interference with both their work and funding.

The preponderance of evidence indicates that IQ tests measuring general intelligence are crossculturally valid. There is little or no evidence of population-specific cultural effects apart from the obvious example of language bias.[189] For example, Robert Sternberg et al. found that the IQ of 12- to 15-year-old Kenyans predicted school grades at about the same level as they do in the West.[190] IQ also predicted university performance equally well in African and non-African engineering students in South Africa in a 2004 study.[191] Salgado et al. (2003) demonstrated the international generalizability of general mental ability across 10 member countries of the European Community and differences in a nation’s culture, religion, language, socioeconomic level or employment legislation did not affect the predictive validity of IQ tests.[192]

The Bell Curve has often been argued to embellish the view that IQ is inheritable. (Nature argument.) However, recent studies have argued that IQ itself is in fact malleable due to conditions of nuture. [193]

Policy implications

See also: Intelligence and public policy

Public policy implications of IQ and race research are one of the greatest sources of controversy surrounding this issue. Regardless of the source of the gap, most educators agree that it must be addressed. They often advocate equitable funding for education.[194][195]

Some proponents of a genetic interpretation of the IQ gap, such as Template:A(Y)ref and Template:A(Y)ref, have sometimes argued that their interpretation does not in itself demand any particular policy response: while a conservative/libertarian commentator[196] may feel the results justify, for example, reductions in affirmative action, a liberal commentator may argue from a Rawlsian point of view (that genetic advantages are undeserved and unjust) for substantial affirmative action.[197] Since all races have representatives at all levels of the IQ curve, this means any policy based on low IQ affects members of all races.

While not specifically race-related, policies focused on geographical regions or nations may have disproportionate influences on certain racial groups and on cognitive development. Differences in health care, nutrition, regulation of environmental toxins, and geographic distribution of diseases and control strategies between the developing world and developed nations have all been subjects of policies or policy recommendations (see Health and intelligence).

Melvin Konner, professor of anthropology and associate professor of psychiatry and neurology at Emory University, called Bell Curve a "deliberate assault on efforts to improve the school performance of African-Americans". "This book presented strong evidence that genes play a role in intelligence but linked it to the unsupported claim that genes explain the small but consistent black-white difference in IQ. The juxtaposition of good argument with a bad one seemed politically motivated, and persuasive refutations soon appeared. Actually, African-Americans have excelled in virtually every enriched environment they have been placed in, most of which they were previously barred from, and this in only the first decade or two of improved but still not equal opportunity. It is likely that the real curves for the two races will one day be superimposable on each other, but this may require decades of change and different environments for different people. Claims about genetic potential are meaningless except in light of this requirement."[198]

Finally, Gregory Stock, writes that germinal choice technology may one day be able to select or change directly alleles found to influence intelligence or racially identifying traits (such as skin color; see gene SLC24A5), making them susceptible to biotechnological intervention.[199]

End material

See also

Notes

  1. ^ Andor, L. E., ed. Aptitudes and Abilities of the Black Man in Sub-Saharan Africa: 1784-1963: An Annotated Bibliography. Johannesburg: National Institute for Personnel Research, 1966.
  2. ^ "Race as Biology Is Fiction, Racism as a Social Problem Is Real: Anthropological and Historical Perspectives on the Social Construction of Race." by Audrey Smedley and Brian D. Smedley[1]
  3. ^ Thirty Years of Research on Race Differences In Cognitive Ability. p. 240
  4. ^ Black-White-East Asian IQ differences at least 50% genetic, major law review journal concludes
  5. ^ Ceci, S. J. (1990). On intelligence more or less: A bioecological treatise on intellectual development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall
  6. ^ Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns
  7. ^ There are no public-policy implications: A reply to Rushton and Jensen (2005) Robert Sternberg
  8. ^ Entman, Robert M. and Andrew Rojecki The Black Image in the White Mind: Media and Race in America 2001
  9. ^ Darwin's Athletes: how sport has damaged Black America and preserved the myth of race By John Milton Hoberman. ISBN 0395822920
  10. ^ Social Darwinism, Scientific Racism, and the Metaphysics of Race Rutledge M. Dennis The Journal of Negro Education, Vol. 64, No. 3, Myths and Realities: African Americans and the Measurement of Human Abilities (Summer, 1995), pp. 243-252
  11. ^ A History of Race/ism Produced By: Tim McCaskell Toronto District School Board
  12. ^ Jalata, Asafa 1954- "Race and Ethnicity in East Africa (review)" Africa Today - Volume 48, Number 4, Winter 2001, pp. 134-136 Indiana University Press
  13. ^ The Invention of the White Race By Chantal Mouffe, Theodore (Theodore W.) Allen
  14. ^ Media, Stereotypes and the Perpetuation of Racism in Canada by James Crawford

    Indians were seen as a homogeneous group of savages despite the fact that individual groups varied extensively and had several well developed social systems. Black people were also portrayed as savage, uncivilized and having low intelligence. By creating these social constructs, expansion into North America was justified.

  15. ^ Alexander Thomas and Samuell Sillen (1972). Racism and Psychiatry. New York: Carol Publishing Group.
  16. ^ Samual A. Cartwright, "Diseases and Peculiarities of the Negro Race", DeBow's Review—Southern and Western States, Volume XI, New Orleans, 1851
  17. ^ Eugenics: America's Darkest Days
  18. ^ Francis Galton:British Psychologist
  19. ^ Social Darwinism, Scientific Racism, and the Metaphysics of Race Rutledge M. Dennis The Journal of Negro Education, Vol. 64, No. 3, Myths and Realities: African Americans and the Measurement of Human Abilities (Summer, 1995), pp. 243-252
  20. ^ Template:AYref; Template:AYref
  21. ^ Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref
  22. ^ Outcome-Based Tyranny: Teaching Compliance While Testing Like A State IQ tests administered to the American Expeditionary Forces (AEF) in World War I. Anthropological Quarterly - Volume 76, Number 4, Fall 2003, pp. 715-730
  23. ^ Porteus, Stanley. The Psychology of a Primitive People, 1931.
  24. ^ Racial Inequality: Fact or Myth W. O. Brown, The Journal of Negro History, Vol. 16, No. 1. (Jan., 1931), pp. 49
  25. ^ Killing the Black Body: Race, Reproduction, and the Meaning of Liberty by Dorothy Roberts. Page 63. December 1998 ISBN 0679758690
  26. ^ According to historian of psychology Graham Richards there was widespread critical debate within psychology about the conceptual underpinnings of this early race difference research (Template:AYref). These include Estabrooks (1928) two papers on the limitations of methodology used in the research; Dearborn and Long’s (1934) overview of the criticisms by several psychologists (Garth, Thompson, Peterson, Pinter, Herskovits, Daniel, Price, Wilkerson, Freeman, Rosenthal and C.E. Smith) in a collection they edited and Klineburg, who wrote three major critiques, one in 1928, and two in 1935. Richards also notes that with over a 1000 publications within psychology during the interwar years there had been a large internal debate. Towards the end of the time period almost all those publishing, including most of those who began with a pro-race differences stance, were firmly arguing against race differences research. Richards regards the scientific controversy to be dead at this point, although he also suggests reasons for its re-emergence in the late nineteen sixties.
  27. ^ Template:AYref
  28. ^ Template:AYref; Template:AYref, pp. 45–54
  29. ^ Template:AYref
  30. ^ a b Explaining Race Differences in IQ: The Logic, the Methodology, and the Evidence American Psychologist, November 1984, Brian Mackenzie. Mackenzie writes of Jensen's hereditarian position as a "genetic model", in contrast to a "jointly genetic/environmental" model. Jensen often uses the term "partly-genetic" to describe his position, even though his views aren't seen as congruent with the "jointly genetic/environmental" model described by Mackenzie.
  31. ^ George Bush: The Unauthorized Biography by Webster Griffin Tarpley and Anton Chaitkin, 1992 Executive Intelligence Review, Chapter 11
  32. ^ George Bush: The Unauthorized Biography by Webster Griffin Tarpley and Anton Chaitkin, 1992 Executive Intelligence Review, Chapter 11
  33. ^ {{This paragraph includes excerpts from William Shockley; however editors of this page have expressed concern over the lack of citations at that article. A request for citation has been placed there. Please refer to discussion page before further editing etc}}
  34. ^ Template:AYref
  35. ^ A Genetic and Cultural Odyssey: The Life and Work of L. Luca Cavalli-Sforza by Linda Stone, pages 76, 168 ISBN 0231133960.
  36. ^
    "What of the latest currents of thought? Are they likely to lead to, or at least encourage, further distortions of social policy? The indications are not all encouraging. Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray published a book in 1994 clearly directed at policy, just as Jensen and others had in the 1960s and 1970s. The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life (New York: Free Press, 1994) teamed a psychologist with a conservative policy advocate to try to prove that both the class structure and the racial divide in the United States result from genetically determined differences in intelligence and ability."
    "Their general assertions about genes and IQ were not very controversial, but their speculations on race were something else again."
    "Also in the 1990s, Phillipe Rushton has tried to couch racial differences in IQ in a theory drawn from evolutionary biology. This theory takes the concepts of r and K selection, crudely useful when applied to a vast range of living creatures considered on a continuum, and apply it to subtle differences in skull form, mental test results, and sexual behavior within our one species. This theory has no academic legitimacy and little relationship to real evolutionary theory, but it taints the whole Darwinian enterprise, strongly recalling the “scientific anthropology” of the era of slavery."
    "The reality is quite different. As argued by George Armelagos in his Presidential Address to the American Association of Physical Anthropologists (“Race, Reason and Rationale,” Evolutionary Anthropology 4, 1995, pp. 103–109) race itself is a dubious concept for the human species. Obviously it is sociologically meaningful, but even in the social realm it is a constantly moving target with little or no core biological legitimacy."The Tangled Wing Biological Constraints on the Human Spirit Times Books Pub: 2002 ISBN 0-7167-4602-6
  37. ^ The Tangled Wing: Biological Constraints on the Human Spirit Times Books, Pub Date: Jan. 2002. ISBN 0-7167-4602-6 By Kevin Konner
  38. ^ Statement on "Race" and Intelligence American Anthropological Association
  39. ^ a b 2 Scholarly Articles Diverge On Role of Race in Medicine By NICHOLAS WADE Published: March 20, 2003] New York Times
  40. ^ Template:AYref, Template:AYref (given in Template:AYref's summary, p.599)
  41. ^ Template:AYref, Template:AYref (given in Template:AYref's summary, p. 599)
  42. ^ It is well established that within-population genetic diversity is greatest within Sub-Saharan Africa, and decreases with distance from Africa. One study estimates that only 6.3% of the total human genetic diversity is explained by race. (See: The Biological Meaning of “Race” by Matt Riese) This value is comparable to other reports which find that on average approximately 85% of genetic variation occurs within populations. In a hypothetical situation with two populations and a single gene with two alleles, this is equivalent to allele frequencies of 30% + 70% in one population and 70% + 30% in the other. Thus, using this single gene to classify individuals into populations would result in a 30% misclassification rate.
  43. ^ Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, The concept and measurement of race and their relationship to public health: a review focused on Brazil and the United States
  44. ^ (Diamond 1997/99, p.21).
  45. ^ Template:AYref, Template:AYref. Neil Risch argues: "One could make the same arguments about sex and age! . . you can undermine any definitional system. . . In a recent study. . . we actually had a higher discordance rate between self-reported sex and markers on the X chromosome [than] between genetic structure [based on microsatellite markers] versus [racial] self-description, [which had a] 99.9% concordance. . . So you could argue that sex is also a problematic category. And there are differences between sex and gender; self-identification may not be correlated with biology perfectly. And there is sexism. And you can talk about age the same way. A person's chronological age does not correspond perfectly with his biological age for a variety of reasons, both inherited and non-inherited. Perhaps just using someone's actual birth year is not a very good way of measuring age. Does that mean we should throw it out? . . . Any category you come up with is going to be imperfect, but that doesn't preclude you from using it or the fact that it has utility" (Template:AYref).
  46. ^ Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref: "If enough markers are used... individuals can be partitioned into genetic clusters that match major geographic subdivisions of the globe".
  47. ^ Template:AYref
  48. ^ Bindon, Jim. University of Alabama. "Post World War II". 2005. August 28, 2006
  49. ^ How "Caucasoids" Got Such Big Crania and Why They Shrank
  50. ^ Intelligence, Race, and Genetics Robert J. Sternberg, Elena L. Grigorenko, and Kenneth K. Kidd Yale University
  51. ^ 'Beyond the Bell Curve: Toward a Model of Talent and Character Development Serge Madhere The Journal of Negro Education, Vol. 64, No. 3, Myths and Realities: African Americans and the Measurement of Human Abilities (Summer, 1995), pp. 326-339
  52. ^ J Clin Psychol. 1991 Sep;47(5):698-702.
  53. ^ Predictive validity of two short-forms of the WPPSI: a 3-year follow-up study.
  54. ^ The Predictive Value of IQ Sternberg, Robert J. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly - Volume 47, Number 1, January 2001, pp. 1-41
  55. ^ Text of the APA Task Force Report, "Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns"
  56. ^ See Template:AYref.
  57. ^ Carraher, Carraher, and Schliemann (1985) studied a group of Brazilian street children. The investigation found that the same children who are able to do the mathematics needed to run their street businesses were often unable to do mathematics in a formal setting. See: Street Mathematics and School Mathematics By Terezinha Nunes, David William Carraher, Analucia Dias Schliemann ISBN 0521388139
  58. ^ Mind in Context: Interactionist Perspectives on Human Intelligence By Robert J. Sternberg, Richard K. Wagner
  59. ^ Standardization of the Panga Munthu Test-A Nonverbal Cognitive Test Developed in Zambia Ravinder Kathuria, Robert Serpell The Journal of Negro Education, Vol. 67, No. 3, Assessment in the Context of Culture and Pedagogy (Summer, 1998), pp. 228-241
  60. ^ Template:AYref; Template:AYref; Template:AYref
  61. ^ Rushton, J. P. (2006). "Lynn Richard, Race Differences in Intelligence: An Evolutionary Analysis, Washington Summit Books, Augusta, GA (2005) ISBN 1-59368-020-1, 318 pages., US$34.95". Personality and Individual Differences. 40 (4): 853–855. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.10.004. {{cite journal}}: Cite has empty unknown parameter: |month= (help)
  62. ^ Lynn, R. and Vanhanen, T. (2002). IQ and the wealth of nations. Westport, CT: Praeger. ISBN 0-275-97510-X
  63. ^ Top Colleges Take More Blacks, but Which Ones?
  64. ^ Shades of gray in black enrollment
  65. ^ Crippled by Their Culture OpinionJournal, WSJ.com
  66. ^ http://www.vdare.com/misc/rushton_african_iq.htm
  67. ^ PEOPLE ARE POOR AT CROSS-RACE FACIAL APA News Release December 3, 2000
  68. ^ Minority and Cross-Cultural Aspects of Neuropsychological Assessment By F. Richard Ferraro Page 90 ISBN 9026518307
  69. ^ Children's Ability to Recognize Other Children's Faces Saul Feinman, Doris R. Entwisle Child Development, Vol. 47, No. 2 (Jun., 1976), pp. 506-510
  70. ^ a b Other-Race Face Perception D. Stephen Lindsay, PhilipC. Jack, Jr.,and Marcus A.Christian. Journal of Applied Psychology Cite error: The named reference "otherrace" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page).
  71. ^ Diamond &Carey, 1986; Rhodeset al.,1989
  72. ^ Convergent Trends in Black-White Test-Score Differentials in the U.S.: A Correction of Richard Lynn Min-Hsiung Huang and Robert M. Hauser 2000
  73. ^ Reviewed in Template:AYref. Data from the NLSY as reported in figure adapted from Template:AYref, p. 288.
  74. ^ How Heritability Misleads about Race
  75. ^ Gene variant may depress IQ of males
  76. ^ Link between gene and performance IQ
  77. ^ Gene may affect IQ in males, scientists say Dallas Daily News
  78. ^ Parents pass on genes for reasoning and memory NewScientist.com
  79. ^ A World of Difference: Richard Lynn Maps World Intelligence Gene Expression
  80. ^ Microcephalin, a Gene Regulating Brain Size, Continues to Evolve Adaptively in Humans Gene Expressions
  81. ^ a b c d Template:AYref
  82. ^ Racial equality in intelligence: Predictions from a theory of intelligence as processing Joseph F. Fagan and Cynthia R. Holland. Intelligence Volume 35, Issue 4, July-August 2007, Pages 319-334
  83. ^ PBS Jencks Interview "If we change the names of the tests, they still measure the same thing but it wouldn't convey this idea that somehow you've gotten the potential of somebody when you measured their IQ. And I think that creates a big bias, because the people who do badly on the tests are labeled as people with low potential in many people's minds and they sometimes even believe that about themselves."
  84. ^ Template:AYref "... we find it hard to see how anyone reading these studies with an open mind could conclude that innate ability played a large role in the black-white gap."
  85. ^ Black Americans reduce the racial IQ gap: Evidence from standardization samples William T. Dickens and James R. Flynn. Oct. 2006
  86. ^ Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref
  87. ^ Template:AYref
  88. ^ Template:AYref
  89. ^ Race, Categorisation and Educational Achievement British Journal of Sociology of Education, Vol. 10, No. 2 (1989), pp. 195-214
  90. ^ Fitting into Categories or Falling Between Them? Rethinking ethnic classification Alastair Bonnett, Bruce Carrington. British Journal of Sociology of Education Volume 21, Number 4 / December 1, 2000 Pages:487 - 500
  91. ^ The Effects of Stereotype Threat on the Standardized Test Performance of College Students J Aronson, CM Steele, MF Salinas, MJ Lustina - Readings About the Social Animal, 8th edition, E. Aronson
  92. ^ Steele, C. M. and Aronson, J. (Nov 1995). "Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69 (5): 797-811.
  93. ^ Racial Identity and Academic Achievement
  94. ^ Review of Evidence Relating to Effects of Desegregation on the Intellectual Performance of Negroes I Katz - American Psychologist, 1964
  95. ^ Race and IQ TIME. Monday, Sep. 07, 1970
  96. ^ Sackett, P. R., Hardison, C. M. and Cullen, M. J. (Apr 2005). "On Interpreting Research on Stereotype Threat and Test Performance". American Psychologist 60 (3): 271-272. DOI:10.1037/0003-066x.60.3.271.
  97. ^ African Americans and high blood pressure: the role of stereotype threat. Blascovich J, Spencer SJ, Quinn D and Steele C. Department of Psychology, University of California, Santa Barbara 93106, USA.
  98. ^ Converging Evidence That Stereotype Threat Reduces Working Memory Capacity Toni Schmader and Michael Johns 2003
  99. ^ Stereotype Threat Undermines Intellectual Performance by Triggering a Disruptive Mental Load 2004 Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.
  100. ^ Reading level attenuates differences in neuropsychological test performance between African American and White elders JENNIFER J. MANLY, DIANE M. JACOBS, PEGAH TOURADJI, SCOTT A. SMALL and YAAKOV STERN
  101. ^ Acculturation, Reading Level, and Neuropsychological Test Performance Among African American Elders Jennifer J. Manly‌, Desiree A. Byrd‌, Pegah Touradji‌, Yaakov Stern‌
  102. ^ Cancellation test performance in African American, Hispanic, and White elderly DESIREE A. BYRD, PEGAH TOURADJI, MING-XIN TANG and JENNIFER J. MANLY
  103. ^ School Quality and the Black-White Achievement Gap Eric A. Hanushek and Steven G. Rivkin 2006
  104. ^ Cross-cultural Effects on IQ Test Performance: A Review and Preliminary Normative Indications on WAIS-III Test Performance Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology Volume 26, Number 7 / October 2004
  105. ^ When Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of Racial Discrimination? A Social Science Perspective by Roslyn Arlin Mickelson University of North Carolina at Charlotte
  106. ^ Effect of Children's Ethnicity on Teachers' Referral and Recommendation Decisions in Gifted and Talented Programs Journal article by Negmeldin Alsheikh, Hala Elhoweris, Pauline Holloway, Kagendo Mutua; Remedial and Special Education, Vol. 26, 2005
  107. ^ (Salend, Garrick Duhaney, & Montgomery, 2002; Townsend, 2002)
  108. ^ Racial Inequity in Special Education. Losen, Daniel J., Ed.; Orfield, Gary, Ed. Harvard Education Publishing Group.
  109. ^ (Gay, 2000; Irvine & Armento, 2001; Ladson-Billings, 1994, 2001)
  110. ^ The Effects of African American Movement Styles on Teachers' Perceptions and Reactions Journal article by Scott T. Bridgest, Audrey Davis Mccray, La Vonne I. Neal, Gwendolyn Webb-Johnson; Journal of Special Education, Vol. 37, 2003
  111. ^ Color-Blind Ellis Cose. Page 50
  112. ^ a b Inequality by Design: Cracking the Bell Curve Myth by Claude S. Fischer, Michael Hout, Martín Sánchez Jankowski, Samuel R. Lucas, Ann Swidler, and Kim Vos. Page 192. (The footnotes given are also from this book.) Cite error: The named reference "bell myth" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page).
  113. ^ The Bell Curve and many other places.
  114. ^ Church Academic Achievement
  115. ^ Richard Lynn discussed in Benson Ireland's 'Low' IQ
  116. ^ Lynn et al. Home Background
  117. ^ Klich Aboriginal Cognition and Psychological Science; Clark and Halford, Does Cognitive Style Account for Cultural Differences?
  118. ^ Ogbu, Minority Education and Caste
  119. ^ Verster and Prinsloo, The Diminishing Test Performance Gap
  120. ^ Raven, The Raven Progressive Matrices esp fig. 2
  121. ^ Kugelmass et al., Patterns of Intellectual Ability
  122. ^ Das and Khurana, Catse and Cognitive Processes
  123. ^ Adamovic, Intellectual Development and Level of Knowledge in Gypsy Pupils
  124. ^ Shimahara, Social Mobility and Education
  125. ^ SAGE, School Psychology
  126. ^ Joel Wiesen, "An Annotated List of Many Possible Reasons for the Black-White Mean Score Differences Seen With Many Cognitive Ability Tests: Notes to File," Applied Personnel Research, March 18, 2005.
  127. ^ http://psychology.uwo.ca/faculty/rushtonpdfs/PPPL1.pdf Thirty Years of Research on Race Differences in Cognitive Ability
  128. ^ http://www.innovations-report.de/html/berichte/studien/bericht-43536.html Black-White-East Asian IQ differences at least 50% genetic, major law review journal concludes
  129. ^ A Genome-Wide Scan of 1842 DNA Markers for Allelic Associations With General Cognitive Ability: A Five-Stage Design Using DNA Pooling and Extreme Selected Groups
  130. ^ Brain May Still Be Evolving, Studies Hint
  131. ^ The ongoing adaptive evolution of ASPM and Microcephalin is not explained by increased intelligence
  132. ^ Normal variants of Microcephalin and ASPM do not account for brain size variability
  133. ^ The race myth, Joseph Graves, page 183 ISBN 0452286581
  134. ^ http://homepage.mac.com/harpend/.Public/AshkenaziIQ.jbiosocsci.pdf
  135. ^ [2]
  136. ^ http://www.astarshop.com/j_dis.pdf
  137. ^ [3]
  138. ^ On the high intelligence and cognitive achievements of Jews in Britain
  139. ^ Lynn, R. and Longley, D. (2006). "On the high intelligence and cognitive achievements of Jews in Britain." Intelligence, 34, 541-547.
  140. ^ Thomas Volken, "The Impact of National IQ on Income and Growth."
  141. ^ Template:AYref
  142. ^ Richard Nisbett argues in his 2004 The Geography of Thought that some of these regional differences shaped lasting cultural traits, such as the collectivism required by East Asian rice irrigation, compared with the individualism of ancient Greek herding, maritime mercantilism, and money crops wine and olive oil (pp. 34-35).
  143. ^ This theory is discussed by Template:AYref (pp. 435-437), Template:AYref and Template:AYref in general and by both Template:AYref and Steve Sailer with respect to Guns, Germs, and Steel. See Race and intelligence (Explanations)#Rushton's application of r-K theory. .. Template:AYref state generally that "a number of recent studies have detected more signals of adaptation in non-African populations than in Africans, and some of those studies have conjectured that non-Africans might have experienced greater pressures to adapt to new environments than Africans have" (Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref).
  144. ^ {http://www.gifted.uconn.edu/siegle/research/Correlation/Intelligence.pdf Text of the APA consensus statement
  145. ^ How do you compare pages 28-29 ISBN 0399529519 based on Lapsley and Enright , ' The effect of social desirability, intelligence, and milieu on an American validation of the conservatism scale'
  146. ^ THE PORTRAYALS OF MINORITY CHARACTERS IN ENTERTAINING ANIMATED CHILDREN’S PROGRAMS
  147. ^ Media Portrayals of Major League Baseball Pitchers
  148. ^ Patricia J. Williams: "Racial Ventriloquism". The Nation. June 17, 1999. Retrieved June 11. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |accessdate= (help); Unknown parameter |accessyear= ignored (|access-date= suggested) (help)
  149. ^ Entman, Robert M. and Andrew Rojecki The Black Image in the White Mind: Media and Race in America. 2001
  150. ^ America's Mishandling of the Donovan McNabb-Rush Limbaugh Controversy
  151. ^ The Portrayal of Race, Ethnicity and Nationality in Televised International Athletic Events
  152. ^ Jackson Assails Press On Portrayal of Blacks (NYT)
  153. ^ Spike Lee discusses racial stereotypes
  154. ^ Darwin's Athletes: how sport has damaged Black America and preserved the myth of race By John Milton Hoberman ISBN 0395822920
  155. ^ "White Men Can't Jump": Evidence for the Perceptual Confirmation of Racial Stereotypes Following a Basketball Game Jeff Stone, ‌W. Perry, ‌John M. Darley. Basic and Applied Social Psychology 1997, Vol. 19, No. 3, Pages 291-306
  156. ^ The Ball Curve: Calculated Racism and the Stereotype of African American Men Ronald E. Hall Journal of Black Studies, Vol. 32, No. 1 (Sep., 2001), pp. 104-119
  157. ^ Katz/Braly(1933), Karlins, Coffman, & Walters, 1969; Maykovich, 1972
  158. ^ Leo W. Jeffres, K. Kyoon Hur (1979) White Ethnics and their Media Images Journal of Communication 29 (1), 116–122.
  159. ^ "A staple of American humor about the UK is the population's bad teeth."
  160. ^ "Why Villains in Movies Have English Accents". January 15, 2003
  161. ^ Deconstructing Whiteness: Irish Women in Britain Mary J. Hickman, Bronwen Walter Feminist Review, No. 50, The Irish Issue: The British Question (Summer, 1995), pp. 5-19 doi:10.2307/1395487
  162. ^ Kerry Soper, "Performing 'Jiggs': Irish Caricature and Comedic Ambivalence toward Asøsimilation and the American Dream in George McManus's Bringing Up Father." Journal of the Gilded Age and Progressive Era 4.2 (2005): 72 pars. 30 Mar. 2007 online.
  163. ^ The Movies, Race, and Ethnicity: Jews
  164. ^ Not Crazy About Goy Crazy By Lynn Melnick
  165. ^ HEREDITY, ENVIRONMENT, AND RACE DIFFERENCES IN IQ: A Commentary on Rushton and Jensen (2005) Richard E. Nisbett Psychology, Public Policy, and Law June 2005 Vol. 11, No. 2, 302-310
  166. ^ Fog Watch: The New Racist Onslaught
  167. ^ Social Darwinism, scientific racism, and the metaphysics of race Journal of Negro Education, The, Summer 1995 by Dennis, Rutledge M
  168. ^ Thin Ice: Stereotype Threat and Black College Students by Claude M. Steele
  169. ^ Template:AYref: "One [issue raised by readers of this article] is that misinterpretation of research is regrettably all too common and thus that documenting misinterpretations in one single domain is of limited interest. Our response is that we are singling out this domain because the issue at stake is of such importance and because the interpretive errors are so rampant and so systematic" (p. 11).
  170. ^ Other researchers have extended these results to other groups (e.g., gender, age) (p. 11).
  171. ^ pp. 10-11.
  172. ^ "We can only speculate as to causes of the mischaracterization of the Steele and Aronson (1995) findings in these various media. . . A factor contributing to not noticing the adjustment may be the appeal of the misinterpreted findings (i.e., the conclusion that eliminating stereotype threat eliminates African American–White differences). Finding mechanisms to reduce or eliminate subgroup differences is an outcome that we believe would be virtually universally welcomed. Thus, research findings that can be interpreted as contributing to that outcome may be more readily accepted with less critical scrutiny" (p. 11).
  173. ^ Template:AYref.
  174. ^ (1995) [4]
  175. ^ Cite error: The named reference APA-report was invoked but never defined (see the help page).
  176. ^ (American Psychologist, January 1997)
  177. ^ Murray lists race differences in brain size, along with "IQ in sub-Saharan Africa, the results of transracial adoption studies, the correlation of the black-white difference with the g-loadedness of tests, regression to racial means across the range of IQ, or other relevant data" among the arguments omitted from the task force report.[5]
  178. ^ The authors of the report agreed that IQ scores have high predictive validity for individual differences in school achievement. They confirmed the predictive validity of IQ for adult occupational status, even when variables such as education and family background have been statistically controlled. They agree that individual differences in intelligence are substantially influenced by genetics (75% in adults). Consistent with Herrnstein and Murray's findings, they state there is little evidence to show that childhood diet influences intelligence except in cases of severe malnutrition.
  179. ^ "In a field where so many issues are unresolved and so many questions unanswered, the confident tone that has characterized most of the debate on these topics is clearly out of place. The study of intelligence does not need politicized assertions and recriminations; it needs self-restraint, reflection, and a great deal more research. The questions that remain are socially as well as scientifically important. There is no reason to think them unanswerable, but finding the answers will require a shared and sustained effort as well as the commitment of substantial scientific resources. Just such a commitment is what we strongly recommend."
  180. ^ Template:AYref, Template:AYref, Template:AYref pp. 44-47.
  181. ^ Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray, "Race, genes and I.Q.—an apologia: the case for conservative multiculturalism," The New Republic 211, no. 11 (October 1994): 27.
  182. ^ http://www.lrainc.com/swtaboo/taboos/apa_01.html
  183. ^ e.g., Sternberg, 2003, pp. 386-387
  184. ^ e.g., Sternberg, 2003, pp. 386-387
  185. ^ Hunt & Carlson, in press
  186. ^ http://www.vdare.com/rushton/061207_iq.htm
  187. ^ Frank, Reanne, The Misuse of Biology in Demographic Research on Racial/Ethnic Differences: A Reply to van den Oord and Rowe, Demography - Volume 38, Number 4, November 2001, pp. 563-567
  188. ^ Tatum, Beverly Daniel (1997). Why are all the black kids sitting together in the cafeteria? And other conversations about race. New York: BasicBooks. ISBN 9780465091270.
  189. ^ http://www.charlesdarwinresearch.org/PRSL2007.pdf
  190. ^ Sternberg, R. J., Nokes, C., Geissler, P. W., Prince, R., Okatcha, F., Bundy, D. A. & Grigorenko, E. L. 2001 The relationship between academic and practical intelligence: a case study in Kenya. Intelligence 29, 401–418.
  191. ^ Construct validity of Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices for African and non-African engineering students in South Africa.
  192. ^ Salgado, J. F., Anderson, N., Moscoso, S., Bertua, C. & Fruyt, F. D. 2003 International validity generalization of GMA and cognitive abilities: a European community meta-analysis. Pers. Psychol. 56, 573–605.
  193. ^ Hawkes, N. (2007) 'Is there any truth in the claim that black people are less intelligent than whites?' The Times (Accessed Saturday October 20th 2007)
  194. ^ Achieving Equitable Education in Calhoun County
  195. ^ Educational Studies: A Jrnl of the American Educ. Studies Assoc.
  196. ^ For example, the policy recommendations of The Bell Curve were denounced by many.[citation needed] Template:AYref wrote: "We can imagine no recommendation for using the government to manipulate fertility that does not have dangers. But this highlights the problem: The United States already has policies that inadvertently social-engineer who has babies, and it is encouraging the wrong women. If the United States did as much to encourage high-IQ women to have babies as it now does to encourage low-IQ women, it would rightly be described as engaging in aggressive manipulation of fertility. The technically precise description of America's fertility policy is that it subsidizes births among poor women, who are also disproportionately at the low end of the intelligence distribution. We urge generally that these policies, represented by the extensive network of cash and services for low-income women who have babies, be ended. (p. 548)" Two year later the 1996 U.S. welfare reform substantially cut these programs. In a discussion of the future political outcomes of an intellectually stratified society, they stated that they: "fear that a new kind of conservatism is becoming the dominant ideology of the affluent - not in the social tradition of an Edmund Burke or in the economic tradition of an Adam Smith but ’conservatism’ along Latin American lines, where to be conservative has often meant doing whatever is necessary to preserve the mansions on the hills from the menace of the slums below. (p. 518)"Moreover, they fear that an increasing welfare will create a "custodial state": "a high-tech and more lavish version of the Indian reservation of some substantial minority of the nation’s population. They also predict increasing totalitarianism: It is difficult to imagine the United States preserving its heritage of individualism, equal rights before the law, free people running their own lives, once it is accepted that a significant part of the population must be made permanent wards of the states. (p. 526)"
  197. ^ Template:AYref
  198. ^ The Tangled Wing Biological Constraints on the Human Spirit by Melvin Konner, 2nd edition, p. 428
  199. ^ Gregory Stock argues "current debates about whether some of the differences among ethnic and racial groups are cultural or biological will soon become irrelevant, given the coming [malleability of biological traits]" (Template:AYref, p. 194; race and intelligence discussed on pp. 44-47).

References

Collective Statements

Review Papers

Others

Template:Race and intelligence